Original Paper
Abstract
Background: The efficacy of digital meditation is well established. However, the extent to which the benefits remain after 12 weeks in real-world settings remains unknown. Additionally, findings related to dosage and practice habits have been mixed, and the studies were conducted on small and homogeneous samples and used a limited range of analytical procedures and meditation techniques. Findings related to the predictors of adherence are also lacking and may help inform future meditators and meditation programs on how to best structure healthy sustainable practices.
Objective: This study aimed to measure outcome change across a large and globally diverse population of meditators and meditations in their naturalistic practice environments, assess the dose-response relationships between practice habits and outcome change, and identify predictors of adherence.
Methods: We used ecological momentary assessment to assess participants’ well-being over a 14-month period. We engineered outcomes related to the variability of change over time (equanimity) and recovery following a drop in mood (resilience) and established the convergent and divergent validity of these outcomes using a validated scale. Using linear mixed-effects and generalized additive mixed-effects models, we modeled outcome changes and patterns of dose-response across outcomes. We then used logistic regression to study the practice habits of participants in their first 30 sessions to derive odds ratios of long-term adherence.
Results: Significant improvements were observed in all outcomes (P<.001). Generalized additive mixed models revealed rapid improvements over the first 50-100 sessions, with further improvements observed until the end of the study period. Outcome change corresponded to 1 extra day of improved mood for every 5 days meditated and half-a-day-faster mood recovery compared with baseline. Overall, consistency of practice was associated with the largest outcome change (4-7 d/wk). No significant differences were observed across session lengths in linear models (mood: P=.19; equanimity: P=.10; resilience: P=.29); however, generalized additive models revealed significant differences over time (P<.001). Longer sessions (21-30 min) were associated with the largest magnitude of change in mood from the 20th session onward and fewer sessions to recovery (increased resilience); midlength sessions (11-20 min) were associated with the largest decreases in recovery; and mood stability was similar across session lengths (equanimity). Completing a greater variety of practice types was associated with significantly greater improvements across all outcomes. Adhering to a long-term practice was best predicted by practice consistency (4-7 d/wk), a morning routine, and maintaining an equal balance between interoceptive and exteroceptive meditations.
Conclusions: Long-term real-world digital meditation practice is effective and associated with improvements in mood, equanimity, and resilience. Practice consistency and variety rather than length best predict improvement. Long-term sustainable practices are best predicted by consistency, a morning routine, and a practice balanced across objects of focus that are internal and external to the body.
doi:10.2196/43358
Keywords
Introduction
Background
The efficacy of mindfulness meditation through digital and nondigital media is well established [
- ]. However, the extent to which real-world benefits remain after the first 8 to 12 weeks remains unknown [ ]. It is also unclear how much meditation at what level of consistency (dosage and practice habits) is most effective for whom [ ], whether this changes over the course of a meditator’s journey as they gain experience, and how these factors interact with the types of meditation practiced [ ].In the context of rising rates of mental illness, stress, burnout, and absenteeism worldwide, digital meditation presents itself as a promising supplement to assist in the management of these problems and improve overall well-being [
, ]. However, although digital meditation appears efficacious in structured, controlled, and prescribed environments [ ], there are no large-scale studies that have assessed tolerance, adherence, and effectiveness in the real world. Therefore, a detailed real-world analysis of the effects of dose and practice habits across a diverse range of meditators and meditation types is required to understand whether digital meditation confers long-term benefits.Thus far, the findings on the effects of dose and practice habits in controlled environments have been mixed. A meta-analysis found that brief mindfulness-based programs were as effective as higher-dose mindfulness-based programs for psychological distress in working nonclinical populations [
]. In clinical populations, a review of between-session home practice frequency and outcome change identified a dose-response relationship in only half of the reviewed studies [ ]. A meta-regression by Strohmaier [ ] found dose-response relationships for mindfulness compared with controls associated with program intensity, use, and teacher-trainee face-to-face contact. However, no relationships were found between session length and psychological outcomes after controlling for baselines and making false discovery rate corrections [ ]. In a 2-week–long randomized controlled trial (RCT), Strohmaier et al [ ] found an effect of mindfulness meditation on trait mindfulness, depression, anxiety, and stress in beginner meditators. Interestingly, evidence favored shorter session lengths for trait mindfulness and anxiety [ ].Even though the effects of dosage are unclear, specific components of meditation also contribute to its effectiveness. For example, different types of meditation contain distinct cognitive and therapeutic mechanisms [
, ] and direct the meditator to different objects of focus [ ]. For example, mindfulness of the breath, body scans, and many secular practices place the object of focus internally in the body (interoception), whereas alternative and spiritually and religiously oriented meditations often place the object of focus externally (exteroception), such as on compassionate thoughts for others, a positive self-affirmation, or a mantra [ ]. Britton [ ] proposed that these different objects of focus may have implications for long-term efficacy and adherence. For example, body-focused interoceptive meditations encourage growth in the insular cortex, a region thought to be involved in bodily perception and emotion. Although interoceptive meditations are more common in digital meditation (and the main focus of research in controlled study environments), a disproportionate practice of these meditations might risk oversensitizing a meditator to bodily and emotional states [ , ]. On the contrary, balancing these practices with those that place the object of focus outside the body may mitigate this risk of oversensitization and help a meditator break patterns of internal rumination [ ].In the same way that meditations vary in their attributes, so do the individual and cultural characteristics of meditators [
, ]. Exploring the use of digital meditation across a large and culturally diverse sample is required to understand the relationship between a meditation practice and outcome change. Indeed, the inconsistency of findings related to dose and practice may be attributable to the use of small and nonrepresentative samples. Furthermore, nonlinear dose-response relationships may have been overlooked in previous studies that used linear techniques, whereas the use of infrequently measured outcomes may have masked the relationship between dosage and outcomes [ ]. Large heterogeneous samples, nonlinear modeling techniques, and ecological momentary assessment (EMA; a method that frequently captures how an individual feels over time) [ ] can be used to overcome these limitations.Recently, the use of EMA in mobile meditation studies has increased and demonstrated positive changes in well-being, depression, emotion regulation, and mindfulness [
, - ] and has shown an increased sensitivity to detect change compared with traditional assessment techniques [ ]. A single-item mood (SIM) scale is a common form of EMA that asks a user how they feel and measures their responses on a Likert scale. Given its open-ended nature, it can also be used to create secondary measures. For example, equanimity, a state of psychological stability and composure [ ], can be engineered by measuring a meditator’s SIM variability, whereas resilience, a psychological state improved by mindfulness [ ] and predictor of well-being [ , ], can be measured by looking at the time it takes for a meditator’s SIM to recover following a decrease from its norm. In addition to studying EMA-based outcomes, understanding adherence and its predictors may help inform future meditators and meditation programs on how to best structure healthy, sustainable practices. Previous research has shown that personality characteristics, depressive symptoms, motivation, trait mindfulness, and the use of EMA itself are positively associated with adherence [ - ]. However, the relationship between a meditator’s early practice habits and adherence remains unknown.Objectives
Given the increasing global burden of disability arising from mental health disorders [
], the general efficacy of mindfulness meditation in clinical and nonclinical populations [ ], and the ease with which meditative practices can be completed in solitary or group settings, using EMA to answer questions related to practice habits, dosage, effectiveness, and adherence may help personalize mindfulness meditation for meditators [ ]. Therefore, we examined the longitudinal association between the total number of meditation sessions completed both overall and per week, session length, the length of time between sessions, ratios of interoceptive and exteroceptive practices, changes in meditator mood, equanimity, resilience, and the odds of long-term practice adherence using the mobile meditation app Insight Timer. The goal was to measure patterns of change over time and determine the optimal dose, frequency, and duration across a diverse set of meditators and meditations while accounting statistically for user characteristics and covariates that might confound these relationships.Methods
Recruitment
New English-speaking Insight Timer users who were aged ≥18 years, completed onboarding (registered for an account and provided information on their characteristics and previous meditation experience), and used Insight Timer’s mood check-in feature were sent an in-app message containing a plain-language statement and informed consent form. Only those who consented to having their in-app use data used for peer-reviewed research purposes were included in the analysis. Insight Timer also collects routine survey data from users for product review, safety, quality assurance, and research purposes. After providing informed consent for deidentified app use analysis, users were linked to an additional survey in the in-app message and completed the even-minded state of mind subscale of the Two-Factor Equanimity Scale. These users were provided with a second plain-language statement and consent form in which they consented to the use of their survey data for quality audit and peer-reviewed research purposes as well as the linking of their survey responses to their app use data. The in-app message containing the plain-language statement, informed consent statement, and user survey was made available to users from November 24, 2021, to December 22, 2021.
Outcome Variables and Data Structure
SIM Scale
Users’ mood was measured in each meditation session using a 5-level single-item Likert SIM scale (a form of EMA), an alternative to traditional clinical assessment that affords repeated measurements in real-life settings, leading to increased reliability and ecological validity [
]. Users were presented with a screen asking the following—“How are you today?”—and responded by selecting 1 of 5 icons representing terrible, bad, okay, good, or great. See Figure S1 in [ - ] for a visualization and the supplementary methods section in for more information. In addition, see Figure S2 in for a visualization of Insight Timer’s new user onboarding flow at the time of data collection.Shifted Mood Scores and Practice Periods
As users reported their mood before each meditation, we shifted each mood score forward so that each session predicted the following session’s score. For example, meditation session 1 predicted the score for meditation session 2 and so forth. This also allowed for the use of the first score as a baseline covariate. See Figure S3 in
for an example of this structure. Although this structure increased our confidence in causative relationships, this became invalidated when there was a large gap between sessions. For instance, if a user did not meditate for 2 weeks, it is implausible that the meditation session from 14 days before would have a predictive effect on the meditator’s current mood. Instead, whatever confounds caused this break would likely be responsible (eg, increased work stress and time spent on other well-being activities). To circumvent this problem, we developed the notion of practice periods.We defined a practice period as any consecutive period of meditation with no longer than 7 days between sessions. We chose this value as it captured 90% of the distribution for days between sessions and naturally fit within a weekly cycle of everyday habits (Figure S4 in
). For example, if a user completed 20 meditations, and across them, there was a range of breaks between 1 and 7 days, all 20 of these meditations would be in 1 practice period. If the user then did not meditate for >7 days and then completed another 20 sessions, all completed within ≤7 days, these subsequent sessions would be considered as meditations completed in a second practice period.Therefore, when we shifted users’ mood scores, we shifted them for each user within their practice period. Thus, each time a user did not meditate for >7 days and returned to meditate, we removed and controlled for their first mood score as a baseline covariate in that practice period and then brought forward every other mood score for prediction. This way, we could control for confounding variables in between sessions without the need to know precisely what they were while mitigating the risk of misattributing outcome change to meditation sessions when significant gaps were present between them. See Figure S3 in
for more information.Equanimity
Many authors suggest using equanimity, a psychologically relevant mental state derived from Buddhist literature, as an outcome in contemplative research [
, ]. This state can be defined as [ ] “An even-minded state of mind or dispositional tendency toward all experiences or objects, regardless of their affective valence (pleasant, unpleasant, or neutral) or source” [ ]. According to this definition, equanimity is associated with less emotional interference [ ], greater emotional stability and its underlying neural correlates [ ], higher levels of inner peace [ ], and reduced general stress [ ].As this state comprises composure and stability, we should expect that, when a meditator’s equanimity increases, their SD of mood should decrease. Therefore, we measured equanimity based on the rolling 5-session SD of the user’s mood (5 sessions represented the median number of sessions within a practice period per user; Figure S5 in
). We turned this into a standardized measure by sign flipping these values (decreasing SD was now measured by increasing, not decreasing, values) and normalized each value on a range from 1 to 5. This was done to have an equanimity measure on the same scale as the primary mood outcome and to intuitively observe increasing equanimity with positive values and decreasing equanimity with negative values. We established this measure’s convergent and divergent validity using each item on the even-minded state of mind subscale from the Two-Factor Equanimity Scale [ ].Resilience: Number of Sessions to Recovery Following a 1-Point Mood Drop
To measure resilience, we created a variable that measured the number of sessions to recovery following a 1-point drop in mood from the meditator’s previous week’s average (see Figure S6 in
for the distribution of SIM measurements per week). Specifically, when a meditator’s mood dropped 1 point below their previous week average, a counter started counting until the user reverted to or above their previous week’s mood. With this outcome, we would expect more resilient users to revert sooner. If more meditation sessions increase a user’s resilience over time, we would expect more sessions to be associated with faster reversion. To avoid confounding this measure by users who churned (ceased to continue meditating), we excluded users who had a 1-point mood drop but had not yet returned for another session. We also established this measure’s convergent and divergent validity using each item on the even-minded state of mind subscale of the Two-Factor Equanimity Scale [ ] ( ).Adherence
To measure adherence, we looked at meditators’ practice habits in their first 30 sessions. We chose this value as it corresponded to the median number of sessions completed. We then defined adhering users as those who reached the top decile (10%) of the session distribution as this corresponded to completing a high number of sessions (≥150 meditation sessions) while retaining an adequate sample size (365/2084, 17.51% of meditators who completed their first 30 sessions; Figure S7 in
).Predictor Variables and Measures
As we were interested in using guided meditations performed by the respondents and related to their SIM, we removed all meditations related to children as it was possible that it was not the parent completing the meditation. We also removed sleep meditations as we could not verify at what point a user may have fallen asleep relative to the length of a track, which would have confounded any dose-response relationships. As users were free to complete mood check-ins as desired and all in-app use was logged, all remaining data were used in the analyses.
To calculate participants’ total number of meditation sessions, we cumulatively summed the number of sessions completed by each participant. We then harmonized this variable into days per week and days since the last meditation variables using each session’s time stamp and then harmonized the length of each session from seconds to minutes. To measure the effects of performing different proportions of meditations with an object of focus external to the body (eg, compassion and loving-kindness meditation, henceforth referred to as exteroceptive meditations) compared with an object of focus internal to the body (eg, mindfulness of the breath and body scans, henceforth referred to as interoceptive meditations), we divided the cumulative number of exteroceptive meditations by the cumulative number of interoceptive meditations over time per user (Figure S8 in
). To measure the effects of performing multiple practice types, we summed the number of distinct meditation types performed per meditator as they completed their sessions. Henceforth, these variables are referred to as our predictor variables.To reduce the possibility of spurious outliers, we applied trimming using the box plot method to the total number of meditation sessions completed and each meditation session length such that responses greater than 291 meditation sessions and 30 minutes were removed from the analyses. Total meditation sessions and session lengths were harmonized to the nearest 5 minutes and 5 meditation sessions (Figures S9 and S10 in
) to reduce sparsity and minimize estimation errors.In subsequent analyses, we used these variables to predict our mood, equanimity, and resilience outcomes. In all analyses, we controlled for the following onboarding covariates: why a meditator was meditating (managing stress, anxiety, or sadness or improving their well-being), content preferences, previous meditation experience, and age. From the meditators’ mood check-ins, we controlled for their baseline mood for each practice period and their mood attributions. We also controlled for content-related covariates that could confound the relationship between dosage and outcome change. These included the ratings and number of ratings, play counts, meditation topic sentiment (eg, positivity focused, problem focused, or techniques), worldview of the meditation (eg, scientific or secular, Buddhist, religious, or of other spiritual origins), and practice type of each meditation (eg, Vipassana, loving-kindness, guided imagery, and mindfulness of the breath; see the supplementary methods 3 section in
for more information). Henceforth, these variables are referred to as the full covariate set.Statistical Analysis
Outcome data were hierarchical in nature (users came from 103 countries) and contained repeated measures (multiple guided meditation sessions and outcome measurements per user). For linear models of outcome change, we used hierarchical mixed-effects models with longitudinal mood, equanimity, and resilience scores as dependent variables. We used random effects to nest each participant in their continent of use while fitting random intercepts for each participant. Next, we used fixed effects to model our predictor variables while controlling for the full covariate set.
We then repeated these analyses using generalized additive mixed models to observe nonlinear relationships for each outcome. The model used penalized thin-plate regression splines with parametric regressors to control for each covariate. We plotted the fitted smoothed coefficients with 95% CIs for the number of completed meditation sessions by the number of days meditated per week, days since the last meditation session, length of each session (our practice habit variables), ratio of exteroceptive to interoceptive sessions, and number of distinct practice types completed. To ensure adequate sample sizes at higher session time points and practice habits, we calculated the relative frequencies of each time point for each level of our practice habit variables and used them to derive session time point cutoffs for our generalized additive mixed models (see the supplementary methods 4 section, Tables S1-S3, and Figure S9 in
).Finally, we used logistic regression to model adherence, regressing our predictor variables and full covariate set from the first 30 meditation sessions onto a binary outcome representing whether a meditator reached their 150th session.
Ethics Approval
This study was approved by the University of Adelaide ethics committee (approval H-2023-111).
Informed Consent and Resources
All participants provided informed consent. Mental health and crisis resources were provided to all participants via the in-app message, the survey, and Insight Timer’s website and mobile app [
].Results
Sample Characteristics
Overall, 10,409 participants from 103 countries took part in the study between May 18, 2021, and August 14, 2022. During this time, they completed 289,630 meditation sessions with associated mood check-ins for a total of 3,331,079 minutes meditated across 1392 unique practice type combinations. Users had an average age of 37.5 (SD 12.6) years and checked in and meditated an average of 44.2 (SD 61.9; median 19, IQR 6-55) times at an average of 1.5 (SD 0.5; median 1.3, IQR 1.74-3.69) sessions per week with an average of 1 (SD 0.8; median 1.2, IQR 0.3-1.6) day since their last meditation. A mean of 5.3 (SD 3.8; median 4, IQR 2-8) practice periods was completed, with an average of 21.8 (SD 23.5; median 16, IQR 6.9-28.9) days between them.
In total, 68.41% (7121/10,409) of users were using Insight Timer to manage anxiety, 65.28% (6795/10,409) were using it to manage stress, 36.69% (3819/10,409) were using it to manage sadness, and 63.17% (6575/10,409) were using it to improve their well-being (multiple selections were permitted to users during onboarding). Of these 10,409 participants, 2033 (19.53%) had no experience of meditation, 2457 (23.6%) had experience with web-based meditation courses, 7282 (69.96%) had experience with meditation apps, 2911 (27.97%) had experience with local meditation classes, 1490 (14.31%) had experience with mentoring, and 1455 (13.98%) had experience at meditation retreats. See
and for sample characteristics, the supplementary results in for additional participant characteristics and outcome intercorrelations, Table S4 in for participant counts and mood scores by country, and Tables S5 and S6 in for the even-minded state of mind subscale of the Two-Factor Equanimity Scale subsample characteristics.Full mood score sample | Equanimity score sample | Resilience score sample | ||||||
Participants, n (%) | Mean (SD) | Participants, n (%) | Mean (SD) | Participants, n (%) | Mean (SD) | |||
Age (years) | 10,409 (100) | 37.45 (12.62) | 5475 (52.59) | 37.65 (12.64) | 1972 (18.94) | 38.41 (12.53) | ||
Baseline mood score | 10,409 (100) | 3.34 (0.72) | 5475 (52.59) | 3.4 (0.8) | 1972 (18.94) | 3.46 (0.66) | ||
Number of practice periods | 10,409 (100) | 5.26 (3.84) | 5475 (52.59) | 4.39 (3.51) | 1972 (18.94) | 7.82 (3.97) | ||
Days between practice periods | 10,409 (100) | 21.82 (23.5) | 5475 (52.59) | 15.09 (24.83) | 1972 (18.94) | 14.57 (14.17) | ||
Number of mood check-ins | 10,409 (100) | 3.4 (0.64) | 5475 (52.59) | 3.53 (0.64) | 1972 (18.94) | 3.56 (0.51) | ||
Equanimity score | 5475 (52.59) | 4.04 (0.48) | 5475 (52.59) | 4.04 (0.48) | 1967 (18.89) | 4.01 (0.38) | ||
Resilience score | 1972 (18.94) | 1.55 (0.31) | 1967 (18.89) | 1.55 (0.31) | 1972 (18.94) | 1.55 (0.31) | ||
Number of meditation sessions | 10,409 (100) | 44.24 (61.85) | 5475 (52.59) | 62.97 (69.72) | 1972 (18.94) | 125.45 (77.41) | ||
Session length (min) | 10,409 (100) | 11.45 (4.11) | 5475 (52.59) | 11.53 (4.29) | 1972 (18.94) | 11.68 (3.73) | ||
Days per week | 10,409 (100) | 1.48 (0.51) | 5475 (52.59) | 2.13 (0.65) | 1972 (18.94) | 2.08 (0.49) | ||
Days since last meditation | 10,409 (100) | 1.07 (0.76) | 5475 (52.59) | 2.05 (1.06) | 1972 (18.94) | 1.62 (0.37) | ||
Rating score | 10,409 (100) | 4.72 (0.12) | 5475 (52.59) | 4.72 (0.13) | 1972 (18.94) | 4.74 (0.1) | ||
Rating count | 10,409 (100) | 180,000 (190,000) | 5475 (52.59) | 170,000 (210,000) | 1972 (18.94) | 150,000 (160,000) | ||
Play count | 10,409 (100) | 4,300,000 (5,100,000) | 5475 (52.59) | 4,100,000 (5,800,000) | 1972 (18.94) | 3,900,000 (4,600,000) | ||
Number of practice types | 10,409 (100) | 4.73 (2.98) | 5475 (52.59) | 5.95 (2.9) | 1972 (18.94) | 7.68 (3.0) | ||
Exteroceptive to interoceptive session ratio | 8636 (82.96) | 4.17 (11.19) | 4862 (46.70) | 5.59 (13.78) | 1837 (17.64) | 8.82 (20.8) |
Full mood score sample | Equanimity score sample | Resilience score sample | |||||||||||||
Participants, (n=10,409), n (%) | Mood, mean (SD) | Participants, (n=5475) n (%) | Mood, mean (SD) | Participants, (n=1972) n (%) | Mood, mean (SD) | ||||||||||
Continent | |||||||||||||||
Africa | 155 (1.49) | 3.52 (0.58) | 94 (1.72) | 3.56 (0.54) | 43 (2.18) | 3.51 (0.47) | |||||||||
Asia | 396 (3.8) | 3.46 (0.78) | 184 (3.36) | 3.52 (0.64) | 58 (2.94) | 3.57 (0.56) | |||||||||
Australia and Oceania | 982 (9.43) | 3.38 (0.65) | 528 (9.64) | 3.46 (0.53) | 172 (8.72) | 3.58 (0.51) | |||||||||
Europe | 2279 (21.89) | 3.33 (0.64) | 1270 (23.2) | 3.41 (0.55) | 473 (23.99) | 3.48 (0.53) | |||||||||
North America | 6256 (60.1) | 3.42 (0.63) | 3193 (58.32) | 3.52 (0.53) | 1141 (57.86) | 3.59 (0.5) | |||||||||
South America | 341 (3.28) | 3.57 (0.63) | 206 (3.76) | 3.63 (0.62) | 85 (4.31) | 3.67 (0.55) | |||||||||
Experience | |||||||||||||||
Local class (yes) | 2911 (27.97) | 3.43 (0.6) | 1557 (28.44) | 3.53 (0.52) | 585 (29.67) | 3.58 (0.5) | |||||||||
Meditation apps (yes) | 7282 (69.96) | 3.43 (0.63) | 3968 (72.47) | 3.51 (0.54) | 1500 (76.06) | 3.56 (0.52) | |||||||||
Mentoring (yes) | 1490 (14.31) | 3.5 (0.62) | 847 (15.47) | 3.58 (0.54) | 324 (16.43) | 3.63 (0.51) | |||||||||
No experience (yes) | 2033 (19.53) | 3.28 (0.68) | 950 (17.35) | 3.4 (0.57) | 275 (13.95) | 3.53 (0.5) | |||||||||
Web-based course (yes) | 2457 (23.6) | 3.47 (0.63) | 1358 (24.8) | 3.56 (0.54) | 518 (26.27) | 3.61 (0.5) | |||||||||
Retreats (yes) | 1455 (13.98) | 3.5 (0.61) | 790 (14.43) | 3.56 (0.53) | 293 (14.86) | 3.62 (0.48) | |||||||||
Practice type | |||||||||||||||
Alternative | 5455 (52.41) | 3.46 (0.6) | 3005 (54.89) | 3.55 (0.55) | 1294 (65.62) | 3.61 (0.5) | |||||||||
Body scan | 403 (3.87) | 3.22 (0.68) | 222 (4.05) | 3.36 (0.49) | 75 (3.8) | 3.31 (0.45) | |||||||||
Breathing meditation | 681 (6.54) | 3.32 (0.71) | 344 (6.28) | 3.41 (0.55) | 76 (3.85) | 3.53 (0.44) | |||||||||
Compassion meditation | 185 (1.78) | 3.24 (0.67) | 104 (1.9) | 3.28 (0.57) | 17 (0.86) | 3.16 (0.52) | |||||||||
Contemplation | 281 (2.7) | 3.32 (0.68) | 170 (3.11) | 3.44 (0.51) | 43 (2.18) | 3.36 (0.51) | |||||||||
Guided imagery | 981 (9.42) | 3.34 (0.7) | 454 (8.29) | 3.44 (0.53) | 108 (5.48) | 3.48 (0.53) | |||||||||
Loving-kindness (metta) | 79 (0.76) | 3.34 (0.67) | 39 (0.71) | 3.51 (0.65) | 6 (0.3) | 3.51 (0.46) | |||||||||
MBCTa or MBSRb | 67 (0.64) | 3.1 (0.66) | 32 (0.58) | 3.34 (0.59) | 4 (0.2) | 3.14 (0.42) | |||||||||
Mindfulness meditation | 1538 (14.78) | 3.32 (0.64) | 628 (11.47) | 3.42 (0.52) | 200 (10.14) | 3.47 (0.53) | |||||||||
Positive affirmations | 479 (4.6) | 3.59 (0.6) | 358 (6.54) | 3.59 (0.53) | 123 (6.24) | 3.7 (0.51) | |||||||||
Relaxation meditation | 131 (1.26) | 3.21 (0.83) | 65 (1.19) | 3.28 (0.53) | 15 (0.76) | 3.3 (0.53) | |||||||||
Vipassana | 129 (1.24) | 3.42 (0.71) | 54 (0.99) | 3.31 (0.39) | 11 (0.56) | 3.2 (0.38) | |||||||||
Meditation worldview | |||||||||||||||
Hinduism | 9 (0.09) | 3.46 (0.84) | 3 (0.05) | 3.33 (0.53) | —c | — | |||||||||
Buddhism | 727 (6.98) | 3.34 (0.68) | 348 (6.36) | 3.45 (0.52) | 42 (2.13) | 3.59 (0.54) | |||||||||
Islam | — | — | — | — | — | — | |||||||||
Christianity | 87 (0.84) | 3.33 (0.63) | 57 (1.04) | 3.55 (0.57) | 15 (0.76) | 3.58 (0.38) | |||||||||
Judaism | 2 (0.02) | 3.83 (0.24) | 2 (0.04) | 3.61 (0.06) | — | — | |||||||||
Modernism | 2485 (23.87) | 3.26 (0.68) | 1056 (19.29) | 3.34 (0.54) | 240 (12.17) | 3.36 (0.49) | |||||||||
Niches | 1184 (11.37) | 3.55 (0.7) | 724 (13.22) | 3.6 (0.57) | 204 (10.34) | 3.72 (0.53) | |||||||||
Other | 4968 (47.73) | 3.43 (0.61) | 2677 (48.89) | 3.53 (0.53) | 1205 (61.11) | 3.57 (0.5) | |||||||||
Taoism | 5 (0.05) | 3.48 (0.86) | 4 (0.07) | 3.66 (0.83) | 1 (0.05) | 3.07 (nan) | |||||||||
Meditation orientation | |||||||||||||||
Niches | 2659 (25.55) | 3.5 (0.64) | 1583 (28.91) | 3.57 (0.56) | 566 (28.7) | 3.63 (0.52) | |||||||||
Positivity based | 3135 (30.12) | 3.54 (0.57) | 1857 (33.92) | 3.59 (0.5) | 725 (36.76) | 3.67 (0.45) | |||||||||
Problem focused | 3319 (31.89) | 3.2 (0.65) | 1454 (26.56) | 3.31 (0.54) | 553 (28.04) | 3.36 (0.52) | |||||||||
Techniques | 1296 (12.45) | 3.41 (0.66) | 581 (10.61) | 3.46 (0.53) | 128 (6.49) | 3.55 (0.49) | |||||||||
Reason for meditating | |||||||||||||||
Anxiety (yes) | 7121 (68.41) | 3.33 (0.63) | 3731 (68.15) | 3.43 (0.53) | 1345 (68.2) | 3.5 (0.51) | |||||||||
Sadness (yes) | 3819 (36.69) | 3.25 (0.65) | 1981 (36.18) | 3.35 (0.53) | 679 (34.43) | 3.43 (0.52) | |||||||||
Stress (yes) | 6795 (65.28) | 3.35 (0.64) | 3529 (64.46) | 3.45 (0.54) | 1268 (64.3) | 3.51 (0.5) | |||||||||
Well-being (yes) | 6575 (63.17) | 3.41 (0.63) | 3500 (63.93) | 3.5 (0.54) | 1273 (64.55) | 3.56 (0.51) |
aMBCT: mindfulness-based cognitive therapy.
bMBSR: mindfulness-based stress reduction.
cNo mode value (content characteristic not the most common for any participant).
Linear Outcome Change
Mood
A higher number of meditation sessions was significantly associated with higher mood scores in the following session. For every 5 meditation sessions completed, mood increased by 0.0005 (P<.001), whereas for every extra day meditated per week, mood increased by 0.0126 (P<.001). A higher baseline mood for each practice period was associated with a higher overall mood within a practice period (β=.1861; P<.001), whereas every extra day between practice periods led to a decrease in mood of 0.0006 (P<.006). A higher ratio of exteroceptive to interoceptive meditations was associated with significantly higher mood scores in subsequent sessions (β=.0034; P=.003), as was a higher variety of practice types (β=.0068; P<.001). There were no significant associations between session length, the number of days since the last meditation session, or the number of practice periods completed and mood. See
for all results.β (95% CI) | P value | Q valuea | |||||
Intercept | 2.931 (2.816 to 3.046) | <.001 | <0.001 | ||||
Baseline mood | .1861 (.1809 to .1913) | <.001 | <0.001 | ||||
Session number | .0005 (.0005 to .0006) | <.001 | <0.001 | ||||
Session length (min) | .0004 (−.0002 to .0010) | .19 | 0.266 | ||||
Days meditated per week | .0126 (.0103 to .0149) | <.001 | <0.001 | ||||
Days since last meditation | −.0011 (−.0029 to .0008) | .26 | 0.351 | ||||
Practice period | .0004 (−.0015 to .0022) | .70 | 0.813 | ||||
Days between practice periods | −.0006 (−.0007 to −.0004) | <.001 | <0.001 | ||||
Exteroceptive to interoceptive session ratio | .0034 (.0012 to .0056) | .003 | 0.005 | ||||
Number of practice types completed | .0068 (.0046 to .0089) | <.001 | <0.001 | ||||
Meditation rating | .0087 (.0052 to .0123) | <.001 | <0.001 | ||||
Number of ratings | −.0125 (−.0219 to −.0031) | .009 | 0.017 | ||||
Meditation play count | .0124 (.0028 to .0220) | .01 | 0.019 | ||||
Age | −.0431 (−.0547 to −.0316) | <.001 | <0.001 | ||||
Reason for meditating | |||||||
Anxiety (yes) | −.1296 (−.1550 to −.1041) | <.001 | <0.001 | ||||
Stress (yes) | −.0407 (−.0653 to −.0162) | .001 | 0.003 | ||||
Sadness (yes) | −.1321 (−.1554 to −.1088) | <.001 | <0.001 | ||||
Well-being (yes) | .0313 (.0089 to .0537) | .006 | 0.012 | ||||
Time of day meditated | |||||||
Night | —b | — | — | ||||
Morning | .0250 (.0149 to .0350) | <.001 | <0.001 | ||||
Day | −.0088 (−.0201 to .0024) | .12 | 0.185 | ||||
Previous experience types | |||||||
No experience | −.0571 (−.1013 to −.0129) | .01 | 0.019 | ||||
Meditation apps | −.0006 (−.0362 to .0350) | .98 | 0.975 | ||||
Web-based course | .0233 (−.0033 to .0499) | .09 | 0.132 | ||||
Local class | −.0125 (−.0386 to .0135) | .35 | 0.465 | ||||
Retreats | .0312 (−.0017 to .0642) | .06 | 0.101 | ||||
Mentoring | .0411 (.0094 to .0728) | .01 | 0.019 | ||||
Meditation orientation | |||||||
Positivity based | — | — | — | ||||
Niches | −.0019 (−.0172 to .0134) | .81 | 0.876 | ||||
Problem focused | −.0538 (−.0624 to −.0453) | <.001 | <0.001 | ||||
Techniques | −.0140 (−.0241 to −.0040) | .006 | 0.012 | ||||
Practice type | |||||||
Breathing meditation | — | — | — | ||||
Body scan | .0047 (−.0181 to .0276) | .68 | 0.785 | ||||
Compassion meditation | −.0202 (−.0399 to −.0005) | .04 | 0.088 | ||||
Contemplation | .0017 (−.0176 to .0210) | .86 | 0.919 | ||||
Guided imagery | −.0069 (−.0214 to .0075) | .35 | 0.504 | ||||
Loving-kindness (metta) | .0110 (−.0138 to .0359) | .39 | 0.542 | ||||
MBCTc or MBSRd | −.0061 (−.0350 to .0228) | .68 | 0.785 | ||||
Mindfulness meditation | .0034 (−.0104 to .0173) | .63 | 0.785 | ||||
Other | .0041 (−.0090 to .0172) | .54 | 0.710 | ||||
Positive affirmations | .0114 (−.0041 to .0269) | .15 | 0.242 | ||||
Relaxation meditation | −.0261 (−.0472 to −.0050) | .02 | 0.033 | ||||
Vipassana | .0054 (−.0195 to .0302) | .67 | 0.785 | ||||
Meditation worldview | |||||||
Hinduism | — | — | — | ||||
Buddhism | .0024 (−.0789 to .0837) | .95 | 0.973 | ||||
Christianity | −.0078 (−.0946 to .0790) | .86 | 0.914 | ||||
Islam | −.1429 (−.4462 to .1605) | .36 | 0.466 | ||||
Judaism | −.0315 (−.1652 to .1023) | .65 | 0.783 | ||||
Modernism | −.0040 (−.0849 to .0769) | .92 | 0.960 | ||||
Niches | .0104 (−.0699 to .0907) | .80 | 0.876 | ||||
Other | .0106 (−.0701 to .0914) | .80 | 0.876 | ||||
Taoism | .0777 (−.0355 to .1908) | .18 | 0.260 | ||||
Mood attributions | |||||||
Exercise | .0439 (.0331 to .0547) | .001 | <0.001 | ||||
Family | .0275 (.0186 to .0364) | <.001 | <0.001 | ||||
Finances | −.0279 (−.0412 to −.0146) | <.001 | <0.001 | ||||
Food or diet | .0265 (.0139 to .0391) | <.001 | <0.001 | ||||
Friends | .0418 (.0313 to .0523) | <.001 | <0.001 | ||||
Health | −.0336 (−.0424 to −.0248) | <.001 | <0.001 | ||||
Relationships | −.0257 (−.0347 to −.0167) | <.001 | <0.001 | ||||
Sleep | .0094 (.0004 to .0184) | .04 | 0.068 | ||||
Spirituality | .0839 (.0733 to .0945) | <.001 | <0.001 | ||||
Study | −.0052 (−.0196 to .0092) | .48 | 0.616 | ||||
Travel | .0506 (.0337 to .0675) | <.001 | <0.001 | ||||
Work | −.0299 (−.0383 to −.0215) | <.001 | <0.001 |
aFalse discovery rate correction for multiple testing.
bVariable reference level.
cMBCT: mindfulness-based cognitive therapy.
dMBSR: mindfulness-based stress reduction.
Equanimity
After controlling for the full covariate set and the rolling 5-session average of mood (β=.0318; P<.001), a similar pattern of change was observed for equanimity. In addition, both equanimity and resilience measures showed convergent and divergent validity using the even-minded state of mind subscale of the Two-Factor Equanimity Scale. See the supplementary results section in
. For every 5 meditation sessions completed, equanimity increased by 0.0006 (P<.001), whereas for every extra day meditated per week, equanimity increased by 0.002 (P=.02). A higher baseline mood for each practice period was associated with lower equanimity within a practice period (β=−.0318; P<.001). In contrast, every extra practice period completed was associated with increasing equanimity (β=.0028; P=.003). For every extra day since the last meditation session within a practice period, equanimity decreased by 0.0121 (P<.001). A higher ratio of exteroceptive to interoceptive meditations was associated with significantly higher equanimity (β=.0012; P<.001), as was a higher variety of practice types (β=.0095; P<.001). No significant effects were observed for days between practice periods and session length. See for all results.β (95% CI) | P value | Q valuea | |||||
Intercept | 2.665 (2.565 to 2.766) | <.001 | <0.001 | ||||
Session time point | .0006 (.0006 to .0007) | <.001 | <0.001 | ||||
Rolling 5-session mood mean | .3802 (.3740 to .3865) | <.001 | <0.001 | ||||
Baseline mood | −.0318 (−.0371 to −.0265) | <.001 | <0.001 | ||||
Session length (min) | −.0005 (−.0010 to .0001) | .10 | 0.210 | ||||
Days per week | .0020 (.0003 to .0038) | .02 | 0.064 | ||||
Days since last meditation | −.0121 (−.0139 to −.0104) | <.001 | <0.001 | ||||
Practice period | .0028 (.0009 to .0046) | .003 | 0.012 | ||||
Days between practice periods | .0001 (−.0002 to .0003) | .51 | 0.642 | ||||
Exteroceptive to interoceptive session ratio | .0012 (.0009 to .0014) | <.001 | <0.001 | ||||
Number of practice types completed | .0095 (.0074 to .0117) | <.001 | <0.001 | ||||
Rating score | −.0009 (−.0039 to .0022) | .58 | 0.710 | ||||
Rating count | −.0146 (−.0243 to −.0048) | .003 | 0.013 | ||||
Play count | .0141 (.0041 to .0242) | .006 | 0.019 | ||||
Age | .0833 (.0708 to .0958) | <.001 | <0.001 | ||||
Reason for meditating | |||||||
Anxiety (yes) | .0211 (−.0065 to .0488) | .13 | 0.240 | ||||
Stress (yes) | .0179 (−.0087 to .0444) | .19 | 0.291 | ||||
Sadness (yes) | −.0021 (−.0276 to .0233) | .87 | 0.957 | ||||
Well-being (yes) | −.0009 (−.0252 to .0235) | .94 | 0.959 | ||||
Time of day meditated | |||||||
Night | —b | — | — | ||||
Morning | .0367 (.0278 to .0455) | <.001 | <0.001 | ||||
Day | −.0026 (−.0127 to .0075) | .62 | 0.722 | ||||
Previous experience types | |||||||
No experience | .0326 (−.0161 to .0813) | .19 | 0.291 | ||||
Meditation apps | −.0064 (−.0453 to .0325) | .75 | 0.840 | ||||
Local class | −.0243 (−.0525 to .0039) | .09 | 0.198 | ||||
Web-based course | −.0185 (−.0472 to .0102) | .21 | 0.302 | ||||
Retreats | −.0094 (−.0450 to .0261) | .60 | 0.722 | ||||
Mentoring | −.0177 (−.0515 to .0161) | .31 | 0.414 | ||||
Meditation orientation | |||||||
Positivity based | — | — | — | ||||
Niches | .0002 (−.0134 to .0138) | .98 | 0.977 | ||||
Problem focused | −.0157 (−.0235 to −.0080) | <.001 | <0.001 | ||||
Techniques | .0070 (−.0022 to .0162) | .13 | 0.240 | ||||
Practice types | |||||||
Breathing meditation | — | — | — | ||||
Body scan | .0173 (−.0035 to .0381) | .10 | 0.210 | ||||
Compassion meditation | .0203 (.0029 to .0376) | .02 | 0.063 | ||||
Contemplation | .0008 (−.0160 to .0175) | .93 | 0.958 | ||||
Guided imagery | .0185 (.0058 to .0313) | .004 | 0.016 | ||||
Loving-kindness (metta) | .0086 (−.0132 to .0305) | .44 | 0.564 | ||||
MBCTc or MBSRd | .0169 (−.0091 to .0429) | .20 | 0.302 | ||||
Mindfulness meditation | .0092 (−.0032 to .0216) | .15 | 0.258 | ||||
Positive affirmations | .0070 (−.0065 to .0205) | .31 | 0.414 | ||||
Relaxation meditation | −.0035 (−.0222 to .0152) | .71 | 0.817 | ||||
Vipassana | .0323 (.0095 to .0551) | .006 | 0.019 | ||||
Other | .0109 (−.0007 to .0225) | .07 | 0.152 | ||||
Meditation worldview | |||||||
Hinduism | — | — | — | ||||
Buddhism | .0414 (−.0287 to .1116) | .25 | 0.354 | ||||
Christianity | .0857 (.0110 to .1604) | .03 | 0.064 | ||||
Islam | −.1201 (−.4030 to .1628) | .41 | 0.532 | ||||
Judaism | .0822 (−.0314 to .1958) | .16 | 0.261 | ||||
Modernism | .0534 (−.0162 to .1230) | .13 | 0.240 | ||||
Niches | .0498 (−.0193 to .1190) | .16 | 0.261 | ||||
Other | .0548 (−.0146 to .1242) | .12 | 0.240 | ||||
Taoism | .0891 (−.0096 to .1877) | .08 | 0.173 | ||||
Mood attributions | |||||||
Exercise | .0024 (−.0069 to .0117) | .62 | 0.722 | ||||
Family | −.0081 (−.0158 to −.0004) | .04 | 0.097 | ||||
Finances | .0006 (−.0110 to .0122) | .92 | 0.958 | ||||
Food | .0172 (.0064 to .0281) | .002 | 0.008 | ||||
Friends | .0006 (−.0084 to .0096) | .90 | 0.957 | ||||
Health | −.0179 (−.0255 to −.0103) | <.001 | <0.001 | ||||
Relationships | −.0248 (−.0326 to −.0170) | <.001 | <0.001 | ||||
Sleep | −.0054 (−.0131 to .0023) | .17 | 0.276 | ||||
Spirituality | .0006 (−.0085 to .0097) | .89 | 0.957 | ||||
Studies | .0164 (.0038 to .0290) | .01 | 0.033 | ||||
Travel | −.0150 (−.0292 to −.0007) | .04 | 0.097 | ||||
Work | .0129 (.0056 to .0201) | <.001 | 0.003 |
aFalse discovery rate correction for multiple testing.
bVariable reference level.
cMBCT: mindfulness-based cognitive therapy.
dMBSR: mindfulness-based stress reduction.
Resilience
A higher number of completed meditations was significantly associated with a lower number of sessions to recovery following a 1-point drop in mood. For every 5 meditation sessions completed, the number of sessions to recovery decreased by −0.0016 (P<.001), whereas for every extra day meditated per week, the number of sessions to recovery decreased by −0.0254 (P=.003). For every extra day since the last meditation session within a practice period, the number of sessions to recovery decreased by −0.0652 (P<.001), whereas for every extra practice period completed, the number of sessions to recovery decreased by −0.0081 (P<.001). After controlling for the number of completed sessions alongside the full covariate set, a higher number of completed practice types was associated with a lower number of sessions to recovery (β=−.0125; P<.001). No significant effects were observed for higher baseline mood, days between practice periods, exteroceptive to interoceptive session ratios, or session length. See
for all results.β (95% CI) | P value | Q valuea | ||
Intercept | 2.003 (1.745 to 2.261) | <.001 | <0.001 | |
Session time point | −.0016 (−.0018 to −.0014) | <.001 | <0.001 | |
Baseline mood | −.0144 (−.0294 to .0006) | .06 | 0.271 | |
Session length (min) | −.0017 (−.0048 to .0014) | .29 | 0.538 | |
Days per week | −.0254 (−.0421 to −.0088) | .003 | 0.028 | |
Days since last meditation | −.0652 (−.0774 to −.0530) | <.001 | <0.001 | |
Practice period | −.0081 (−.0119 to −.0044) | <.001 | <0.001 | |
Days between practice periods | .0012 (−.0061 to .0085) | .75 | 0.885 | |
Exteroceptive to interoceptive session ratio | −.0010 (−.0034 to .0014) | .42 | 0.700 | |
Number of practice types completed | −.0125 (−.0178 to −.0073) | <.001 | <0.001 | |
Rating score | .0018 (−.0108 to .0145) | .78 | 0.885 | |
Rating count | −.0072 (−.0432 to .0289) | .70 | 0.870 | |
Play count | −.0133 (−.0510 to .0245) | .49 | 0.737 | |
Age | −.0098 (−.0228 to .0031) | .14 | 0.358 | |
Reason for meditating | ||||
Anxiety (yes) | .0197 (−.0088 to .0481) | .18 | 0.420 | |
Stress (yes) | −.0151 (−.0425 to .0123) | .28 | 0.538 | |
Sadness (yes) | .0219 (−.0047 to .0484) | .11 | 0.304 | |
Well-being (yes) | −.0051 (−.0305 to .0202) | .69 | 0.870 | |
Time of day | ||||
Night | —b | — | — | |
Day | .0008 (−.0397 to .0413) | .97 | 0.986 | |
Morning | −.0094 (−.0396 to .0209) | .54 | 0.784 | |
Previous experience types | ||||
No experience | −.0576 (−.1100 to −.0053) | .03 | 0.244 | |
Meditation apps | −.0380 (−.0784 to .0023) | .07 | 0.271 | |
Local class | −.0023 (−.0308 to .0261) | .87 | 0.926 | |
Web-based course | −.0168 (−.0458 to .0122) | .26 | 0.538 | |
Retreats | .0140 (−.0217 to .0498) | .44 | 0.716 | |
Mentoring | −.0096 (−.0439 to .0246) | .58 | 0.792 | |
Meditation orientation | ||||
Positivity based | — | — | — | |
Niches | .0276 (−.0199 to .0750) | .25 | 0.538 | |
Problem focused | .0000 (−.0317 to .0318) | >.99 | 0.998 | |
Techniques | .0123 (−.0375 to .0621) | .63 | 0.838 | |
Practice types | ||||
Breathing meditation | — | — | — | |
Body scan | −.0060 (−.0832 to .0712) | .88 | 0.926 | |
Compassion meditation | .0101 (−.0595 to .0797) | .78 | 0.885 | |
Contemplation | .0585 (−.0085 to .1256) | .09 | 0.271 | |
Guided imagery | .0530 (.0002 to .1057) | .049 | 0.267 | |
Loving-kindness (metta) | .0985 (−.0256 to .2225) | .12 | 0.326 | |
MBCTc or MBSRd | .1190 (−.0188 to .2568) | .09 | 0.271 | |
Mindfulness meditation | .0302 (−.0240 to .0845) | .28 | 0.538 | |
Other | .0454 (−.0014 to .0921) | .06 | 0.271 | |
Positive affirmations | .0685 (.0054 to .1316) | .03 | 0.244 | |
Relaxation meditation | .1238 (.0015 to .2461) | .047 | 0.267 | |
Vipassana | .1750 (−.0170 to .3669) | .07 | 0.271 | |
Meditation worldview | ||||
Hinduism | — | — | — | |
Buddhism | .0730 (−.1658 to .3119) | .55 | 0.784 | |
Christianity | −.0588 (−.3221 to .2044) | .66 | 0.862 | |
Judaism | −.0208 (−.5908 to .5491) | .94 | 0.975 | |
Modernism | .0415 (−.1940 to .2770) | .73 | 0.885 | |
Niches | .0841 (−.1521 to .3203) | .49 | 0.737 | |
Other | .0685 (−.1664 to .3035) | .57 | 0.792 | |
Taoism | −.2478 (−.8230 to .3273) | .40 | 0.700 | |
Mood attributions | ||||
Exercise | −.0102 (−.0828 to .0623) | .78 | 0.885 | |
Family | .0385 (−.0052 to .0822) | .08 | 0.271 | |
Finances | .0363 (−.0275 to .1001) | .27 | 0.538 | |
Food | −.0482 (−.1219 to .0256) | .20 | 0.463 | |
Friends | −.0585 (−.1236 to .0065) | .08 | 0.271 | |
Health | .0392 (.0024 to .0759) | .04 | 0.244 | |
Relationships | .0144 (−.0235 to .0523) | .46 | 0.720 | |
Sleep | −.0368 (−.0771 to .0036) | .07 | 0.271 | |
Spirituality | .0451 (−.0185 to .1086) | .16 | 0.411 | |
Studies | −.0362 (−.1099 to .0376) | .34 | 0.612 | |
Travel | −.0113 (−.1537 to .1311) | .88 | 0.926 | |
Work | .0147 (−.0208 to .0501) | .42 | 0.700 |
aFalse discovery rate correction for multiple testing.
bVariable reference level.
cMBCT: mindfulness-based cognitive therapy.
dMBSR: mindfulness-based stress reduction.
Adherence
Meditators with a balanced practice (completed 1 exteroceptive meditation for every 1 interoceptive meditation) in their first 30 sessions were 1.73 times as likely to make it to their 150th meditation session (odds ratio 1.73; P=.048) compared with those who predominantly completed interoceptive meditations. Those who meditated 4 to 7 days per week were 2.3 times more likely to make it to their 150th session compared with those who only meditated 1 day per week (P<.001). Compared with those who meditated in the evening, morning and daytime meditators were 2.1 (P=.003) and 1.8 (P=.049) times as likely to make it to the 150th meditation session, respectively. Finally, there were no significant differences in adherence across practice types, orientation, or worldview; the number of practice types completed; or session length. In addition, there were no significant differences in adherence for meditators’ baseline mood, mood attributions, previous experience, or reason for meditating. See
and for all results.OR (95% CI) | P value | Q valuea | |||||
Exteroceptive to interoceptive session ratio | |||||||
0:1 | —b | — | — | ||||
1:1 | 1.730 (1.011-3.004) | .048 | 0.531 | ||||
2:1 | 1.756 (0.9363-3.327) | .08 | 0.531 | ||||
3:1 | 1.743 (0.8532-3.573) | .13 | 0.594 | ||||
4:1 | 1.999 (0.9188-4.347) | .08 | 0.531 | ||||
5:1 | 1.453 (0.5942-3.473) | .41 | 0.952 | ||||
6:1 | 1.056 (0.4546-2.408) | .90 | 0.996 | ||||
7:1 | 0.2100 (0.0106-1.330) | .17 | 0.659 | ||||
8:1 | 1.571 (0.6070-3.992) | .35 | 0.952 | ||||
9:1 | 4.168 (0.7453-23.70) | .10 | 0.594 | ||||
Baseline mood | 1.187 (0.9461-1.494) | .14 | 0.594 | ||||
Session length (min; factorized) | |||||||
5-10 | — | — | — | ||||
10-20 | 0.8517 (0.6016-1.199) | .36 | 0.952 | ||||
20-30 | 1.430 (0.6228-3.146) | .38 | 0.952 | ||||
Days per week (factorized) | |||||||
1 | — | — | — | ||||
2 | 1.222 (0.7497-1.958) | .41 | 0.952 | ||||
3 | 1.253 (0.4680-3.087) | .64 | 0.996 | ||||
4-7 | 2.326 (1.601-3.385) | <.001 | <0.001 | ||||
Days since last meditation (factorized) | |||||||
1 | — | — | — | ||||
2 | 0.5333 (0.2611-1.005) | .07 | 0.531 | ||||
3-4 | 0.5571 (0.1542-1.560) | .31 | 0.952 | ||||
5-7 | 0.0000 (0.0000-189,68+04) | .98 | 0.996 | ||||
Practice period | 0.5978 (0.5361-0.6615) | <.001 | <0.001 | ||||
Days between practice periods | 1.001 (0.992-1.010) | .75 | 0.996 | ||||
Number of practice types completed (factorized) | |||||||
1-4 | — | — | — | ||||
5-8 | 1.026 (0.6486-1.641) | .92 | 0.996 | ||||
9-12 | 1.194 (0.6374-2.241) | .58 | 0.996 | ||||
Rating score | 0.8350 (0.0997-7.466) | .87 | 0.996 | ||||
Rating count | 1.000 (1.000-1.000) | .31 | 0.952 | ||||
Play count | 1.000 (1.000-1.000) | .52 | 0.996 | ||||
Age (years) | 1.025 (1.014-1.037) | <.001 | <0.001 | ||||
Session count | 0.997 (0.973-1.021) | .81 | 0.996 | ||||
Mood score | 1.053 (0.7565-1.467) | .76 | 0.996 | ||||
Reason for meditating | |||||||
Anxiety | 1.087 (0.7738-1.533) | .63 | 0.996 | ||||
Stress | 0.8699 (0.6297-1.204) | .40 | 0.952 | ||||
Sadness | 1.002 (0.7251-1.381) | .99 | 0.996 | ||||
Well-being | 1.035 (0.7648-1.405) | .83 | 0.996 | ||||
Time of day | |||||||
Night | — | — | — | ||||
Day | 1.824 (1.007-3.349) | .049 | 0.531 | ||||
Morning | 2.110 (1.300-3.532) | .003 | 0.059 | ||||
Previous experience types | |||||||
No experience | 0.8550 (0.4625-1.587) | .62 | 0.996 | ||||
Meditation apps | 0.992 (0.6209-1.615) | .97 | 0.996 | ||||
Web-based course | 1.360 (0.966-1.909) | .08 | 0.531 | ||||
Local class | 0.972 (0.6868-1.371) | .87 | 0.996 | ||||
Retreats | 0.996 (0.6490-1.514) | .99 | 0.996 | ||||
Mentoring | 0.958 (0.6274-1.448) | .84 | 0.996 | ||||
Meditation orientation | |||||||
Positivity based | — | — | — | ||||
Niches | 0.7934 (0.4176-1.473) | .47 | 0.996 | ||||
Problem focused | 1.029 (0.7091-1.489) | .88 | 0.996 | ||||
Techniques | 1.513 (0.8783-2.573) | .13 | 0.594 | ||||
Practice types | |||||||
Breathing meditation | — | — | — | ||||
Body scan | 0.9458 (0.2895-2.840) | .92 | 0.996 | ||||
Compassion meditation | 1.659 (0.5185-4.888) | .37 | 0.952 | ||||
Contemplation | 0.8918 (0.2426-2.874) | .85 | 0.996 | ||||
Guided imagery | 1.176 (0.6182-2.261) | .62 | 0.996 | ||||
MBCTc or MBSRd | 0.992 (0.1063-6.167) | .99 | 0.996 | ||||
Mindfulness meditation | 1.118 (0.6316-2.015) | .71 | 0.996 | ||||
Other | 1.225 (0.6897-2.217) | .50 | 0.996 | ||||
Positive affirmations | 1.894 (0.8516-4.196) | .12 | 0.594 | ||||
Relaxation meditation | 0.5310 (0.0253-3.988) | .59 | 0.996 | ||||
Meditation worldview | |||||||
Modernism | — | — | — | ||||
Buddhism | 0.7467 (0.2973-1.714) | .51 | 0.996 | ||||
Christianity | 0.7450 (0.0966-3.775) | .74 | 0.996 | ||||
Niches | 1.492 (0.7093-3.128) | .29 | 0.952 | ||||
Other | 1.219 (0.8067-1.856) | .35 | 0.952 | ||||
Mood attributions | |||||||
Work | 0.9359 (0.6530-1.332) | .72 | 0.996 | ||||
Exercise | 0.7836 (0.4647-1.297) | .35 | 0.952 | ||||
Family | 1.329 (0.9089-1.932) | .14 | 0.594 | ||||
Finances | 1.205 (0.6759-2.099) | .52 | 0.996 | ||||
Food | 0.5441 (0.2757-1.033) | .07 | 0.531 | ||||
Friends | 1.095 (0.6878-1.723) | .70 | 0.996 | ||||
Health | 1.047 (0.7212-1.510) | .81 | 0.996 | ||||
Relationship | 1.106 (0.7478-1.625) | .61 | 0.996 | ||||
Sleep | 1.173 (0.7901-1.728) | .42 | 0.952 | ||||
Spirituality | 0.9386 (0.5828-1.491) | .79 | 0.996 | ||||
Studies | 0.7134 (0.3631-1.337) | .31 | 0.952 | ||||
Travel | 1.034 (0.4491-2.224) | .93 | 0.996 |
aFalse discovery rate correction for multiple testing.
bVariable reference level.
cMBCT: mindfulness-based cognitive therapy.
dMBSR: mindfulness-based stress reduction.
Nonlinear Outcome Change
Building on these findings, we observed significant nonlinear relationships between our predictor and outcome variables. After controlling for the full covariate set, smoothed regression splines revealed that the largest gains in mood, equanimity, and resilience were observed within approximately the first 100 sessions, where up to 50% of improvement was achieved. These improvements were sustained and continued until the 290th session, at which point improvements were still increasing (P<.001 for all outcomes; graph 1 in
A, 2A, and 3A).Improvements in outcomes were noted regardless of the number of days meditated per week. However, compared with a practice of 1 day per week, 4 to 7 days was associated with the highest mood scores in the following sessions for all time points, followed by 2 and 3 days per week (P<.001 for all levels). These observations were further supported when looking at the number of days since a meditator’s last session. Compared with 1 day since the last meditation session (eg, meditating over consecutive days), 2 and 3 to 7 days since the previous sessions were associated with lower mood scores over time (P<.001 and P=.02, respectively; graphs 2 and 3 in
A). See for average mood by days per week and days since last meditation session combinations.A similar pattern was observed for equanimity; however, the largest increases were observed in the first 50 sessions. These improvements were sustained and continued until the 290th session, at which point improvements were still increasing (P<.001). As meditators progressed and completed more sessions, the largest increases in equanimity were observed from the 50th session onward for meditators practicing 4 to 7 days per week compared with those practicing 1 day per week (P<.001). Practicing 2 and 3 days per week followed the same trend (P<.001); however, the effects were smaller. The number of days since the last meditation session followed this same trend. Compared with 1 day since the last session, equanimity scores were significantly lower for 2 and 3 to 7 days (P<.001; graphs 2 and 3 in
A).For the number of sessions to recovery following a drop in mood (resilience), most of the effect was observed within the first 80 to 100 sessions (P<.001). These improvements were maintained through to the 290th session. Recovery times decreased consistently for all levels of days meditated per week and days since the last session. Until the 40th session, this effect was largest for more consistent practices (4-7 d/wk and 1 d since the previous meditation session; P<.001). From the 40th session onward, recovery rates were similar for all levels of practice (graphs 2 and 3 in
A).Although there were no significant relationships between session length and outcome change in the linear models, plotting change over time by session length in nonlinear models revealed significant and varied relationships across outcomes. Meditations of all lengths were associated with improvements in outcomes (P<.001). Shorter and midlength sessions (5-10 and 11-20 min) were associated with higher mood scores until the 20th meditation session, whereas longer sessions (21-30 min) were associated with higher moods from the 20th to the 290th session when compared with 5- to 10-minute sessions (P<.001; graph 4 in
A).Equanimity changes were similar for all session lengths over time; however, shorter sessions were associated with higher equanimity scores from the 80th session onward (P<.001) when compared with longer ones (21-30 min). Faster recovery following a mood drop (resilience) was associated with longer session lengths at all time points (11-20–min sessions: P<.001; 21-30–min sessions: P=.001) when compared with 5- to 10-minute sessions (graph 4 in
A).Finally, we observed significant nonlinear differences for different exteroceptive to interoceptive meditation ratios over time. Compared with those who predominantly completed interoceptive meditations, those who completed a higher portion of exteroceptive meditations (specifically, 2:1 exteroceptive to interoceptive) had higher mood scores from the 80th session onward (P<.001). In addition, a higher number of practice types was associated with higher mood scores from the 80th session onward (5-8 practice types: P<.001; 9-11 practice types: P=.001; graphs 1 and 2 in
C).Discussion
Principal Findings
This study showed meaningful and significant longitudinal associations between digital meditation practice and outcome change in the largest ecologically valid sample to date. After controlling for a large range of covariates, we found that consistency of practice and balance between internally and externally focused meditations were the strongest predictors of outcome change and adherence in linear models. Breaking down meditators’ trajectories by their practice habits in nonlinear models confirmed these findings and revealed further idiosyncrasies across practice habits and outcome changes over time, particularly in relation to session length, ratios of internally to externally focused meditations, and the number of distinct practice types completed.
Overall, we observed an approximate mood score increase of 0.2 to 0.3 over the study period (
and ). Placing this change in the context of real-world mood improvement corresponds to approximately 1 extra day of improved mood for every 5 meditation sessions completed at the end of the study period. For example, if a meditator checked in with a score of 3 (feeling OK) across 5 meditation sessions, this would result in an average mood score of 3. If the same meditator then checked in with a mood score of 3 for 4 out of 5 meditations and a score of 4 (feeling good) for 1 out of 5 meditations, this would correspond to an average mood score of 3.2 for a total change magnitude of 0.2, as was observed over this study period. Given that the study sample comprised a nonclinical population (not specifically targeted to meditators experiencing psychopathology), this degree of improvement is arguably of both statistical and real-world significance.More broadly, our findings tell an interesting story in relation to a meditator’s emotional change over the course of their practice. Specifically, although overall mood increased consistently at all time points, practice consistency (4-7 days meditated per week) first afforded faster mood recovery (increased resilience), which then transitioned to increased mood stability (equanimity) over time (from the 40th session onward). This pattern of change suggests that increased resilience may act as a prerequisite building block for the attainment of increased equanimity later in a meditator’s practice. Interestingly, a similar multiphasic pattern of change was observed in a cohort of participants engaged in a 6-month web-based coaching program. In the first 3 months, larger increases were observed in emotional regulation, prospection, self-awareness, self-efficacy, social connection, and stress management, whereas in the following 3 months, larger increases were observed in life satisfaction, purpose and meaning, and resilience [
].Comparison With Prior Work
Placing these dosage and practice habit findings in the context of previous literature helps disentangle some of the mixed results of previous dose-response studies. For example, the meta-regression by Strohmaier [
] found generally conflicting results across studies, with some favoring longer sessions, others favoring shorter ones, and others finding no significant differences. Following this meta-regression, Strohmaier et al [ ] conducted an RCT finding that shorter meditation sessions were significantly associated with decreased stress in beginner meditators yet observed no differences across psychological outcomes. In our analyses, our linear models aligned with the subset of literature that found no significant differences between session length and overall change. However, when studying and plotting nonlinear curves of change over time, we observed an association between marginally higher mood scores and shorter session lengths over the first 20 sessions of practice. This might explain the findings of Strohmaier et al [ ] given that their RCT was only 2 weeks in duration. Moreover, we then observed the largest statistically significant positive associations between longer meditation sessions and mood change from the 20th session onward. Therefore, the relationship between outcome change and session length may be dependent on how far a meditator has progressed in their current practice. Thus, session length effects may not emerge until a requisite amount of practice has been completed. Future RCTs should aim to extend their window of observation and consider graded progression of dosage as meditators gain experience.Overall, our findings suggest that it was the consistency of practice, not the length of individual sessions, that was the most important predictor of change. Previous studies have found that many beginner meditators have difficulty sustaining a meditative practice when forced to complete longer sessions [
, , ]. Although the consensus is that higher dosages are associated with more favorable outcomes [ ], our results suggest that shorter sessions may be better for beginner meditators and midlength and longer sessions may be better for meditators as they gain experience [ , ]. Moreover, given the self-regulatory nature of digital practice, it seems that some meditators are capable of effectively self-regulating the required practice amounts and session lengths. It may be that those who have less favorable outcomes or churn are attempting to complete patterns of practice beyond their acquired ability. Given this potential dissonance and the high rates of attrition seen in structured programs of longer lengths (eg, mindfulness-based stress reduction and mindfulness-based cognitive therapy programs) [ ], it may be prudent for practitioners to encourage consistency and longevity of practice over length and tailor programs to individuals’ needs.Regarding objects of focus, predominantly completing exteroceptive meditations from the 80th session onward was associated with the highest mood scores (specifically, a ratio of 2:1 exteroceptive to interoceptive meditations). Moreover, a balanced practice of interoceptive and exteroceptive meditations (1:1) was associated with the highest odds of adherence. Studying the characteristics of meditators predominantly completing either meditation type revealed that those meditating for anxiety were significantly less likely to predominantly practice exteroceptive meditations, whereas those meditating for sadness were significantly more likely to predominantly practice exteroceptive meditations (see the supplementary results section in
). An explanation may be that those meditating for anxiety were more likely to practice internally focused meditations as these allow the meditator to manage, decenter, accept, and let go of feelings of anxiety in their body. On the contrary, those meditating for sadness may be more likely to practice externally focused meditations as these allow them to break out of internal states of rumination and reorient themselves to a healthier state of mind. Clinical and neurobiological findings lend support to this view.Mechanistically, deficits in interoception (awareness of internal bodily states) are associated with hypoactivation of the insular cortex, a brain structure that corresponds to perceptual self-awareness [
]. For meditators with heightened anxiety and interoceptive deficits who avoid or fight internal anxiety states, reorienting, accepting, and diffusing these sensations may provide a way to relieve this anxiety and balance insular cortex activation. For example, de Jong et al [ ] found that increases in interoception were associated with greater well-being in patients with chronic pain who avoided sensations in their bodies. Although it is a distinct pathology, it shares significant comorbidity with anxiety disorders [ ]. In addition, avoidance and a lack of decentered acceptance of anxiety states have been shown to moderate anxiety symptoms (known as experiential avoidance) [ - ]. This observation and a subsequent intervention composed of interoceptive awareness combined with acceptance of the bodily experience are core tenants of acceptance and commitment therapy, a clinically validated mindfulness-based intervention targeted at treating anxiety disorders [ - ].In contrast, ruminative behaviors are a common symptom of depression, leaving individuals unable to disengage from the internal processing of maladaptive thought patterns [
]. Research suggests that an altered basal neural resting state, characterized by increased neural activity in brain regions encompassing the default mode network, might account for this increased internal self-focus [ , ]. Moreover, hyperactivation of the insular cortex, a component of the default mode network, has been linked to greater depressive rumination [ ] and increased self-focus [ ] and sadness [ ]. Therefore, practicing exteroceptive meditations may assist those meditating to manage feelings of sadness in breaking maladaptive patterns of self-focus and rumination and reorient to a healthier state of mind [ ].Alternatively, maintaining a balanced practice composed of different mindfulness-related processes might help counterbalance the effects of different practice types [
]. For example, rather than purely focusing on the most common forms of meditation practiced in the West (eg, mindfulness of the breath and body scans [interoceptive practice types]), exploring a broader range of practices that orient the meditator to external stimuli (exteroceptive practices), such as loving-kindness meditation, metta meditation, mantras, or general objects of focus outside the body, may help counterbalance the effects of interoceptive practices and create a healthy balance of focus and insular cortex activation [ ]. When focusing on adherence, our results support this hypothesis, finding that adhering to a long-term practice (reaching at least the 150th session) was best predicted by having a meditation practice that was equally balanced between exteroceptive and interoceptive (1:1) meditations. In addition to objects of focus, we observed that meditators completed 1392 unique practice type combinations across the study period, suggesting that they completed practices that were unique to their preferences and characteristics. For meditators who made it past the 80th session, completing a higher number of practice types was significantly associated with the largest improvements in mood (after controlling for the total number of completed sessions and all other covariates). Thus, those who chose to branch out and explore new practices as they gained experience were likely to observe the largest mood improvement.Future Directions
Although there appears to be a benefit in performing a diverse range of practice types with distinct objects of focus, future works should explore how other components of meditation might effect change. Previous research suggests that different meditations contain distinct cognitive and therapeutic mechanisms (eg, meta-awareness, cognitive reappraisal, and perspective taking), which may help explain their therapeutic efficacy [
]. Identifying and studying which therapeutic mechanisms are most effective for whom and how should be explored in future studies. To do this accurately at scale will require the use of topic modeling and machine learning approaches to identify semantically similar therapeutic mechanisms and objects of focus and for whom they are best suited.In addition to exploring these mechanisms, different meditations contain distinct nontherapeutic components related to their cultural, religious, spiritual, and historical underpinnings that assist in delivering a meditation’s therapeutic components [
]. Understanding how a nontherapeutic component might act to better deliver therapeutic mechanisms may help further explain our findings and improve the personalization of meditations for distinct groups of meditators (eg, from different cultural and religious backgrounds). This could also lead to the creation of entirely new meditative practices that are optimized for distinct mental and emotional needs, cultural and religious characteristics, and other transdiagnostic characteristics of meditators. This is particularly important given emerging evidence suggesting that mental health interventions adapted to cultural norms, religious and spiritual beliefs, and language preferences display superior efficacy and adherence to those of their nonadapted counterparts [ , ].Finally, we observed that those who predominantly completed their meditation practice in the morning were more than twice as likely to adhere to their practice compared with those who practiced in the evening (see the supplementary results in
). Although there is a lack of literature pertaining to optimal practice times, yogic philosophy purports that morning practices completed in the “ambrosial hours” (2.5 hours before sunrise) are best suited to contemplative practice [ ]. Interpreted through a secular lens, this may be due to the stillness and lack of interruption in these hours as well as meditating with the intention of completing practice for practice’s sake rather than in response to stressful phenomena or life events. However, it will be important for future studies to disentangle the extent to which morning practices are causally related to adherence or, instead, are a biproduct of hidden variables (eg, better mental health or personality characteristics) that might explain this result.Strengths
Overall, this is the largest study of longitudinal meditation effectiveness and adherence to date. We maximized the external and ecological validity of our findings through the use of instantaneous in-app reporting and global participation of meditators and validated our resilience and equanimity outcomes on a validated scale [
]. The use of EMA in a naturalistic setting helped address the memory and social desirability biases that have been prevalent in previous research (eg, the retrospective overinflation of session lengths to appeal to instructors and inaccurate recollection of session lengths and counts because of retrospective reporting) [ ]. We were also able to capture, engineer, and interpret a range of meaningful covariates and confounds and assess their impact on the trajectories of meditators over time. We engineered novel features to deal with intermittent patterns of meditation and used nonlinear methods to understand the impacts of dosage and practice habits at particular points in a meditator’s progression.Limitations
This study has a number of limitations. First, those who consented to the use of their in-app and survey data likely represent a nonrandom sample of both Insight Timer meditators and meditators more broadly. In addition, those who used the mood check-in feature again likely represent a more homogeneous subsample of meditators, for example, those who are biased toward meditating explicitly for mood improvement or outcome change. Although we engineered additional features, meditators disinterested in mood tracking and its perceived utility may have opted out of its use, leading to a systematic underrepresentation of users from different facets of the population. Second, although we controlled for meditators’ onboarding characteristics (reason for use, previous experience types, and age), other user characteristics such as gender, socioeconomic status, length of experience in months or years, and psychopathology were not collected during onboarding because of their sensitivity.
To overcome these limitations, future works should aim to study longitudinal effectiveness and dose-response in controlled environments using detailed multi-item measures without the use of a convenience sample and routine data. For example, assessing whether our findings replicate on a random sample of meditators would increase confidence in our findings. Failure to replicate might imply true failure (eg, type 1 error) or the presence of distinct subsets of meditators who have different outcomes based on their transdiagnostic and mental characteristics. Third, although this work has high external and ecological validity given the large sample of diverse global meditators, it is possible that these effects may only be specific to digital meditation rather than in-person meditations guided by a teacher, for example, at a meditation retreat. Future works should aim to assess the generalizability of these findings to face-to-face teaching contexts. Finally, as this work was exploratory in nature, our interpretations relied on the nominal significance of the findings (although corrected P values were provided). Future studies aiming to replicate these findings should define a priori hypotheses and correct for multiple comparisons where necessary.
Conclusions
In conclusion, we found that a higher number of meditation sessions and a consistent daily practice were associated with the highest mood scores, most stable moods (equanimity), and fastest mood recovery over time (resilience). Increased session lengths as meditators gained experience also showed associations with higher mood and faster recovery times. Future works should aim to replicate these findings in controlled study environments and explore other avenues of personalization based on the therapeutic mechanisms of meditations and the clinical and transdiagnostic characteristics of meditators.
Data Availability
The data sets analyzed in this study are not publicly available because of user agreements and international user privacy laws. However, where informed consent is provided by a user, data are made available to Insight Timer research partners.
Authors' Contributions
MC developed the analysis plan, performed all the analyses, and drafted the manuscript. SRC reviewed the analysis plan and results and edited the manuscript. All authors shared final responsibility for the decision to submit the manuscript for publication.
Conflicts of Interest
MC is employed as a research scientist and holds stock options in Insight Timer. SRC received no funding from and is not employed by Insight Timer.
Supplementary information, tables, and figures related to the study’s methodology and results.
DOCX File , 2974 KBReferences
- Spijkerman MP, Pots WT, Bohlmeijer ET. Effectiveness of online mindfulness-based interventions in improving mental health: a review and meta-analysis of randomised controlled trials. Clin Psychol Rev. Apr 2016;45:102-114. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Sommers-Spijkerman M, Austin J, Bohlmeijer E, Pots W. New evidence in the booming field of online mindfulness: an updated meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. JMIR Ment Health. Jul 19, 2021;8(7):e28168. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Goldberg SB, Imhoff-Smith T, Bolt DM, Wilson-Mendenhall CD, Dahl CJ, Davidson RJ, et al. Testing the efficacy of a multicomponent, self-guided, smartphone-based meditation app: three-armed randomized controlled trial. JMIR Ment Health. Nov 27, 2020;7(11):e23825. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Wahbeh H, Oken BS. Internet mindfulness meditation intervention for the general public: pilot randomized controlled trial. JMIR Ment Health. Aug 08, 2016;3(3):e37. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Strohmaier S. The relationship between doses of mindfulness-based programs and depression, anxiety, stress, and mindfulness: a dose-response meta-regression of randomized controlled trials. Mindfulness. Mar 02, 2020;11:1315-1335. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef]
- Schoormans D, Nyklíček I. Mindfulness and psychologic well-being: are they related to type of meditation technique practiced? J Altern Complement Med. Jul 2011;17(7):629-634. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Economides M, Martman J, Bell MJ, Sanderson B. Improvements in stress, affect, and irritability following brief use of a mindfulness-based smartphone app: a randomized controlled trial. Mindfulness (N Y). 2018;9(5):1584-1593. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Virgili M. Mindfulness-based interventions reduce psychological distress in working adults: a meta-analysis of intervention studies. Mindfulness. Dec 13, 2013;6(2):326-337. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef]
- Vettese LC, Toneatto T, Stea JN, Nguyen L, Wang JJ. Do mindfulness meditation participants do their homework? And does it make a difference? A review of the empirical evidence. J Cogn Psychother. Aug 01, 2009;23(3):198-225. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef]
- Strohmaier S, Jones FW, Cane JE. Effects of length of mindfulness practice on mindfulness, depression, anxiety, and stress: a randomized controlled experiment. Mindfulness. 2021;12:198-214. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef]
- Hamilton JP, Furman DJ, Chang C, Thomason ME, Dennis E, Gotlib IH. Default-mode and task-positive network activity in major depressive disorder: implications for adaptive and maladaptive rumination. Biol Psychiatry. Aug 15, 2011;70(4):327-333. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Lemogne C, Delaveau P, Freton M, Guionnet S, Fossati P. Medial prefrontal cortex and the self in major depression. J Affect Disord. Jan 2012;136(1-2):e1-11. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Dahl CJ, Lutz A, Davidson RJ. Reconstructing and deconstructing the self: cognitive mechanisms in meditation practice. Trends Cogn Sci. Sep 2015;19(9):515-523. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Britton WB. Can mindfulness be too much of a good thing? The value of a middle way. Curr Opin Psychol. Aug 2019;28:159-165. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Shapiro DH. A preliminary study of long-term meditators: goals, effects, religious orientation, cognitions. J Transpers Psychol. 1992;24(1):23-39. [FREE Full text]
- Huberty J, Vranceanu AM, Carney C, Breus M, Gordon M, Puzia ME. Characteristics and usage patterns among 12,151 paid subscribers of the calm meditation app: cross-sectional survey. JMIR Mhealth Uhealth. Nov 03, 2019;7(11):e15648. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Moore RC, Depp CA, Wetherell JL, Lenze EJ. Ecological momentary assessment versus standard assessment instruments for measuring mindfulness, depressed mood, and anxiety among older adults. J Psychiatr Res. Apr 2016;75:116-123. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Colombo D, Suso-Ribera C, Fernández-Álvarez J, Cipresso P, García-Palacios A, Riva G, et al. Technology-based ecological momentary assessment in clinical psychology. Compr Clin Psychol. 2022;10:106-120. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef]
- Lahtinen O, Salmivalli C. The relationship between mindfulness meditation and well-being during 8 weeks of ecological momentary assessment. Mindfulness. 2020;11:255-263. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef]
- Enkema MC, McClain L, Bird ER, Halvorson MA, Larimer ME. Associations between mindfulness and mental health outcomes: a systematic review of ecological momentary assessment research. Mindfulness (N Y). Nov 2020;11(11):2455-2469. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Bai S, Elavsky S, Kishida M, Dvořáková K, Greenberg MT. Effects of mindfulness training on daily stress response in college students: ecological momentary assessment of a randomized controlled trial. Mindfulness (N Y). Jun 2020;11(6):1433-1445. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Desbordes G, Gard T, Hoge EA, Hölzel BK, Kerr C, Lazar SW, et al. Moving beyond mindfulness: defining equanimity as an outcome measure in meditation and contemplative research. Mindfulness (N Y). Jan 21, 2014;2014(January):356-372. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Kwak S, Lee TY, Jung WH, Hur JW, Bae D, Hwang WJ, et al. The immediate and sustained positive effects of meditation on resilience are mediated by changes in the resting brain. Front Hum Neurosci. Mar 26, 2019;13:101. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Forbes L, Gutierrez D, Johnson SK. Investigating adherence to an online introductory mindfulness program. Mindfulness. 2018;9(1):271-282. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef]
- Huberty J, Green J, Puzia M, Stecher C. Evaluation of mood check-in feature for participation in meditation mobile app users: retrospective longitudinal analysis. JMIR Mhealth Uhealth. Apr 23, 2021;9(4):e27106. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Canby NK, Eichel K, Peters SI, Rahrig H, Britton WB. Predictors of out-of-class mindfulness practice adherence during and after a mindfulness-based intervention. Psychosom Med. Jul 2021;83(6):655-664. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Vigo D, Thornicroft G, Atun R. Estimating the true global burden of mental illness. Lancet Psychiatry. Feb 2016;3(2):171-178. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Goyal M, Singh S, Sibinga EM, Gould NF, Rowland-Seymour A, Sharma R, et al. Meditation programs for psychological stress and well-being: a systematic review and meta-analysis. JAMA Intern Med. Mar 2014;174(3):357-368. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Schlosser M, Sparby T, Vörös S, Jones R, Marchant NL. Unpleasant meditation-related experiences in regular meditators: prevalence, predictors, and conceptual considerations. PLoS One. May 09, 2019;14(5):e0216643. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Juneau C, Pellerin N, Trives E, Ricard M, Shankland R, Dambrun M. Reliability and validity of an equanimity questionnaire: the two-factor equanimity scale (EQUA-S). PeerJ. Jul 07, 2020;8:e9405. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- van Rijsbergen GD, Burger H, Hollon SD, Elgersma HJ, Kok GD, Dekker J, et al. How do you feel? detection of recurrent major depressive disorder using a single-item screening tool. Psychiatry Res. Dec 15, 2014;220(1-2):287-293. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Killgore WD. The visual analogue mood scale: can a single-item scale accurately classify depressive mood state? Psychol Rep. Dec 1999;85(3 Pt 2):1238-1243. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Abdel-Khalek AM. Measuring happiness with a single-item scale. Soc Behav Pers. Jan 01, 2006;34(2):139-150. [CrossRef]
- de Boer AGEM, van Lanschot JJB, Stalmeier PFM, van Sandick JW, Hulscher JBF, de Haes JCJM, et al. Is a single-item visual analogue scale as valid, reliable and responsive as multi-item scales in measuring quality of life? Qual Life Res. Mar 2004;13(2):311-320. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Hadash Y, Segev N, Tanay G, Goldstein P, Bernstein A. The decoupling model of equanimity: theory, measurement, and test in a mindfulness intervention. Mindfulness. 2016;7(1):1214-1226. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef]
- Ortner CN, Kilner SJ, Zelazo PD. Mindfulness meditation and reduced emotional interference on a cognitive task. Motiv Emot. Nov 20, 2007;31(4):271-283. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef]
- Taylor VA, Grant J, Daneault V, Scavone G, Breton E, Roffe-Vidal S, et al. Impact of mindfulness on the neural responses to emotional pictures in experienced and beginner meditators. Neuroimage. Aug 15, 2011;57(4):1524-1533. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Dambrun M, Ricard M, Després G, Drelon E, Gibelin E, Gibelin M, et al. Measuring happiness: from fluctuating happiness to authentic-durable happiness. Front Psychol. Feb 07, 2012;3:16. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Grossman P, Niemann L, Schmidt S, Walach H. Mindfulness-based stress reduction and health benefits. A meta-analysis. J Psychosom Res. Jul 2004;57(1):35-43. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Ethical considerations in quality assurance and evaluation activities. National Health and Medical Research Council. 2014. URL: https://www.nhmrc.gov.au/about-us/resources/ethical-considerations-quality-assurance-and-evaluation-activities [accessed 2023-05-01]
- Jeannotte AM, Hutchinson DM, Kellerman GR. The time to change for mental health and wellbeing via virtual professional coaching: longitudinal observational study. J Med Internet Res. May 13, 2021;23(7):e27774. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Banerjee M, Cavanagh K, Strauss C. A qualitative study with healthcare staff exploring the facilitators and barriers to engaging in a self-help mindfulness-based intervention. Mindfulness (N Y). 2017;8(6):1653-1664. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Kabat-Zinn J, Chapman-Waldrop A. Compliance with an outpatient stress reduction program: rates and predictors of program completion. J Behav Med. Aug 1988;11(4):333-352. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Kabat-Zinn J. Mindfulness-based interventions in context: past, present, and future. Clin Psychol Sci Pract. May 11, 2006;10(2):144-156. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef]
- de Jong M, Lazar SW, Hug K, Mehling WE, Hölzel BK, Sack AT, et al. Effects of mindfulness-based cognitive therapy on body awareness in patients with chronic pain and comorbid depression. Front Psychol. 2016;7:967. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Asmundson GJ, Katz J. Understanding the co-occurrence of anxiety disorders and chronic pain: state-of-the-art. Depress Anxiety. 2009;26(10):888-901. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- White KS, Brown TA, Somers TJ, Barlow DH. Avoidance behavior in panic disorder: the moderating influence of perceived control. Behav Res Ther. Jan 2006;44(1):147-157. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Rudaz M, Ledermann T, Margraf J, Becker ES, Craske MG. The moderating role of avoidance behavior on anxiety over time: is there a difference between social anxiety disorder and specific phobia? PLoS One. Jul 03, 2017;12(7):e0180298. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Forman EM, Herbert JD, Moitra E, Yeomans PD, Geller PA. A randomized controlled effectiveness trial of acceptance and commitment therapy and cognitive therapy for anxiety and depression. Behav Modif. Nov 2007;31(6):772-799. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Arch JJ, Eifert GH, Davies C, Plumb Vilardaga JC, Rose RD, Craske MG. Randomized clinical trial of cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT) versus acceptance and commitment therapy (ACT) for mixed anxiety disorders. J Consult Clin Psychol. Oct 2012;80(5):750-765. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Swain J, Hancock K, Hainsworth C, Bowman J. Acceptance and commitment therapy in the treatment of anxiety: a systematic review. Clin Psychol Rev. Dec 2013;33(8):965-978. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Sliz D, Hayley S. Major depressive disorder and alterations in insular cortical activity: a review of current functional magnetic imaging research. Front Hum Neurosci. Dec 03, 2012;6:323. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Zhu X, Wang X, Xiao J, Liao J, Zhong M, Wang W, et al. Evidence of a dissociation pattern in resting-state default mode network connectivity in first-episode, treatment-naive major depression patients. Biol Psychiatry. Apr 01, 2012;71(7):611-617. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Grimm S, Ernst J, Boesiger P, Schuepbach D, Boeker H, Northoff G. Reduced negative BOLD responses in the default-mode network and increased self-focus in depression. World J Biol Psychiatry. Dec 2011;12(8):627-637. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Etkin A, Wager TD. Functional neuroimaging of anxiety: a meta-analysis of emotional processing in PTSD, social anxiety disorder, and specific phobia. Am J Psychiatry. Oct 2007;164(10):1476-1488. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Wampold BE. How important are the common factors in psychotherapy? An update. World Psychiatry. Oct 2015;14(3):270-277. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Griner D, Smith TB. Culturally adapted mental health intervention: a meta-analytic review. Psychotherapy (Chic). 2006;43(4):531-548. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Maura J, Weisman de Mamani A. Mental health disparities, treatment engagement, and attrition among racial/ethnic minorities with severe mental illness: a review. J Clin Psychol Med Settings. Dec 2017;24(3-4):187-210. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Khalsa GD, Stauth C, Borysenko J. Meditation As Medicine: Activate the Power of Your Natural Healing Force. Manhattan, NY. Simon & Schuster; 2011.
Abbreviations
EMA: ecological momentary assessment |
RCT: randomized controlled trial |
SIM: single-item mood |
Edited by T Leung; submitted 11.10.22; peer-reviewed by M Kapsetaki, Z Segal; comments to author 05.11.22; revised version received 31.05.23; accepted 26.06.23; published 19.09.23.
Copyright©Micah Cearns, Scott R Clark. Originally published in the Journal of Medical Internet Research (https://www.jmir.org), 19.09.2023.
This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work, first published in the Journal of Medical Internet Research, is properly cited. The complete bibliographic information, a link to the original publication on https://www.jmir.org/, as well as this copyright and license information must be included.