Original Paper
Abstract
Background: Whether and how the uncertainty about a public health crisis should be communicated to the general public have been important and yet unanswered questions arising over the past few years. As the most threatening contemporary public health crisis, the COVID-19 pandemic has renewed interest in these unresolved issues by both academic scholars and public health practitioners.
Objective: The aim of this study was to investigate the impact of communicating uncertainty about COVID-19–related threats and solutions on individuals’ risk perceptions and misinformation vulnerability, as well as the sequential impact of these effects on health information processing and preventative behavioral intentions.
Methods: A 2×2 (threat uncertainty [presence vs absence]×solution uncertainty [presence vs absence]) full-fractional between-subjects online experiment was conducted with 371 Chinese adults. Focusing on the discussion of whether the asymptomatic cases detected during the COVID-19 pandemic would further lead to an uncontrolled pandemic, news articles were manipulated in terms of whether the infectiousness of asymptomatic cases and the means to control the transmission are presented in terms of their certainty or uncertainty. Participants were randomly assigned to one of the four experimental conditions, being instructed to read one news article. After reading the news article assigned, participants were asked to respond to a series of questions to assess their cognitive and behavioral responses.
Results: Individuals were more susceptible to believing false COVID-19–related information when a certain threat and uncertain solution were framed in the news article. Moreover, individuals’ perceptions of crisis severity increased when exposed to news information containing uncertain solutions. Both misinformation vulnerability and perceived severity were positively associated with information processing. Information seeking was positively associated with protective behavioral intention, whereas information avoidance was negatively associated with protective behavioral intention.
Conclusions: Our findings imply that uncertainty, depending on its aspect, can be effectively communicated to the public during an emerging public health crisis. These results have theoretical and practical implications for health communicators and journalists. Given its limited influence on individuals’ cognitive and behavioral responses, uncertainty related to a health threat should be disseminated to meet the public’s expectation of information transparency. However, caution is advised when communicating uncertainty related to potential solutions, as this factor exhibited a mixed impact on individual responses during a crisis.
doi:10.2196/51910
Keywords
Introduction
Background
The question of how to communicate uncertainty to the general public has been raked up during the COVID-19 pandemic, which is considered to be the most threatening public health crisis that emerged over the past 10 years, characterized by a high level of uncertainty. Since its outbreak, news coverage of COVID-19 has largely been emphasizing the “unknowns” about the source, infectivity, treatment, prevention, and control measures of the virus [
]. However, whether (or not) uncertainties should be communicated to the general public remains a controversial issue, given the general low tolerance of the public for uncertainty along with a high expectation for information transparency. On the one hand, uncertainty is an undesirable experience in which people fear losing control of their lives, leading to negative consequences [ , ]. On the other hand, uncertainty may also have positive effects, as some scholars suggest that when uncertainty is perceived, people tend to actively seek for information to ease this feeling, and in this process can gain more information and a deeper understanding of the event [ ]. Thus, it is important to understand whether the uncertainty presented in news articles influences individuals’ cognitive and behavioral intentions during public health emergencies.In actual news framing, uncertainty does not appear as a monolithic entity, and each new challenge presented by COVID-19 involves different aspects (eg, threats and solutions) with varying degrees of uncertainty [
]. According to the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, the uncertainties related to threats and solutions are the two greatest concerns among the public during public health emergencies [ ]. Therefore, this study focused on the uncertainties related to threats and solutions associated with COVID-19 that the news media might (or might not) communicate to the public, with the goal of exploring how this communication of uncertainty might influence individuals’ health behaviors.Impacts of Uncertainty on Risk Perception and Misinformation Vulnerability
Risk perception is always associated with uncertainty in the public health context. It is assumed that individuals will only begin to manage uncertainty through information processing or preventive behaviors when they perceive a given issue to be associated with a certain level of risk [
]. The perceived risk by individuals includes the severity and susceptibility of a public health crisis. Severity refers to the magnitude of harm caused by the threat, whereas susceptibility refers to the probability of occurrence of a threat to a specific subject [ ]. Empirically, Lalot et al [ ] found that individuals’ uncertainty about how the novel coronavirus would affect people significantly increased their perceived threat of the pandemic. Pine et al [ ] found that the partial and changing information would cultivate the uncertainty surrounding COVID-19, which would further influence individuals’ risk perception of the pandemic.Moreover, the potential impact of uncertainty on exacerbating the misinformation effect has been raised as a concern in recent years. Lu et al [
] found that communicating uncertainty about preliminary evidence related to COVID-19 was positively associated with the number of likes and retweets of related misinformation on social media. Consequently, the communication of uncertainty during a pandemic might unexpectedly facilitate engagement with misinformation.To better understand the impact of threat uncertainty and solution uncertainty that are communicated to the general public during a public health crisis, we established the following research question: How, if at all, does the uncertainty frame of a threat and solution exert main and interaction effects on individuals’ risk perceptions and misinformation vulnerability?
Impacts of Uncertainty on Information Processing
Information seeking is regarded as a key communication outcome during a public health crisis, guiding individuals to understand public health issues and consequently adopt appropriate health behaviors (eg, [
, ]). Information seeking refers to individuals’ active information-searching activity through human interaction [ ], online inquiry [ ], and passive observation [ ]. Theoretical and empirical evidence suggests that individuals tend to engage in information-seeking behaviors when experiencing psychological discomfort such as confusion and anxiety resulting from exposure to uncertainty (eg, [ , ]). Although the motivations for information-seeking behaviors according to various demographic characteristics such as age, gender, and health status have been extensively examined in the public health context (eg, [ , ]), little is known about how uncertainty communicated by the media and experts influences individuals’ information-seeking behavior.Case et al [
] pointed out that individuals might also engage in information avoidance to reduce feelings of uncertainty. They found that the mental discomfort that arises due to uncertainty, especially in a health context, could facilitate information-avoidance behaviors. Information avoidance refers to an individual’s absence from or ignorance of information and its source [ ].Thus, we further aimed to understand whether (or not) and how the uncertainty framed in the news would motivate individuals’ information-seeking and information-avoiding behavior differently.
Additionally, the relationship between risk perception and information processing has been documented in the health risk literature. Goodall and Reed [
] suggested that individuals would seek more information when the perceived threat is high. Conversely, Jiang et al [ ] found that a higher risk perception of the COVID-19 pandemic would reduce individuals’ information-seeking behavior. They also found that individuals’ propensity to believe COVID-19 misinformation would also influence their additional information-seeking behavior.Therefore, we sought to examine how, if at all, uncertainty framed in severity and susceptibility influences information processing (ie, information seeking and information avoidance) through risk perceptions and misinformation vulnerability.
Information Seeking and Avoidance Influence Preventive Behaviors
In the context of COVID-19, studies on how organizations and individuals perceive risks during crises have centered on the changes in preventive behaviors during infectious disease outbreaks and how these behavioral shifts can be facilitated by engagement in informational behaviors.
Previous studies suggest that information seeking through different channels and sources is positively associated with preventive behaviors during crises (eg, [
, ]). Individuals who engage in more effortful information seeking and processing are more likely to develop risk-related cognitions, attitudes, and behaviors [ ]. However, the health information environment in a pandemic is often filled with uncertain information, false claims, or even conspiracy theories [ ], which can bias people’s pandemic-related beliefs and impede their adoption of effective actions [ ].Nevertheless, this situation does not imply that refraining from active information seeking is a wise choice. By contrast, the impact of information avoidance on preventive behaviors is not less significant than that of information seeking [
]. While information avoidance minimizes the chances of interaction with unnecessary information, it simultaneously diminishes the opportunities to receive relevant information. From a cognitive perspective, individuals have limited capacity to process information, and if not adequately addressed, the outcome can be information overload [ ]. Avoiding information acquisition may lead individuals to make preventive decisions based on limited information [ ]. Particularly when faced with uncertain information, information avoidance may lead to incorrect preventive behaviors. Therefore, we sought to determine how, if at all, preventative behavioral intentions might be associated with information seeking (1) and information avoidance (2) separately.Methods
Research Design
This study adopted a controlled experiment approach. Based on the question “Will asymptomatic cases lead to an uncontrolled epidemic?” a 2×2 (threat certainty vs uncertainty×solution certainty vs uncertainty) online experiment was designed using asymptomatic cases, an emerging challenge in the COVID-19 epidemic, as a risk scenario. Data were collected in May 2020 through this anonymous online experiment. Participants were recruited from Sojump, which is the largest online survey platform in China. As the context of the experiment, at this time, China was gradually implementing measures to prevent and control the COVID-19 epidemic. However, at the same time, the detection of an increasing number of asymptomatic cases was raising concern. On March 31, 2020, China’s National Health Commission announced that as of the following day (April 1), it would disclose the detection, transition, and management of asymptomatic cases in its daily briefings on the epidemic to respond to these societal concerns in a timely manner [
]. The risk threat posed by the emerging challenge and COVID-19 prevention measures were still being explored and discussed at that time and were fraught with uncertainties. Based on this context, we developed 4 simulated online news reports based on real news coverage and expert interview data; the details of the simulations are provided in with a brief summary in .Although the content orientation of the simulated news coverage differed across the 4 scenarios, the format, word count, structural design, and information volume of the news coverage remained consistent. Participants were randomly assigned to one of the scenarios and were asked to read the simulated news coverage before completing the questionnaire. A total of 592 people participated in the study online and 371 valid questionnaires were obtained after postchecking, including 88 valid questionnaires for condition 1, 99 for condition 2, 88 for condition 3, and 96 for condition 4.
shows the general flow of participants in the study. Participants were from a wide range of age groups. The sample included a relatively equal sex ratio (with 41.80% of the sample identifying as female), and the majority of the participants had an education level of college degree or above (73%). summarizes the main sociodemographic characteristics of the sample.
Condition | Core message in simulated news article |
1: Threat certainty × Solution certainty | The infectiousness of asymptomatic cases is limited and there are adequate guarantees to control the transmission range of asymptomatic cases |
2: Threat certainty × Solution uncertainty | The infectiousness of asymptomatic cases is limited, but the means to control the transmission range of asymptomatic cases are uncertain |
3: Threat uncertainty × Solution certainty | The infectiousness of asymptomatic cases is uncertain, but there are adequate guarantees to control the transmission range of asymptomatic cases |
4: Threat uncertainty × Solution uncertainty | The infectiousness of asymptomatic cases is uncertain and the means to control the transmission range of asymptomatic cases are uncertain |
Characteristics | Participants, n (%) | |
Sex | ||
Male | 216 (58.2) | |
Female | 155 (41.8) | |
Education | ||
Completed PhD degree | 25 (6.7) | |
Completed graduate degree | 88 (23.7) | |
Completed college | 158 (42.6) | |
Completed high school | 49 (13.2) | |
Completed middle school | 51 (13.7) | |
Employment status | ||
Full-time job | 167 (45.0) | |
Part-time job | 12 (3.2) | |
Unemployed or student | 176 (47.4) | |
Retired | 16 (4.3) | |
Monthly income (US $) | ||
<428 | 80 (21.6) | |
428-857 | 98 (26.4) | |
857-1428 | 77 (20.7) | |
>1428 | 60 (16.2) |
Ethical Considerations
The Institutional Review Board office of Renmin University of China approved the research protocol, and responses were collected via Sojump. Before the experiment, participants were asked if they agree to participate in the research voluntarily for receiving compensation. Participants received a reward of RMB 7 (US $1). The final data set is anonymized, without any identifiable private information connected to participants.
Variables
Perceived Severity
Perceived severity was measured with five items adapted from a previous study [
]: (1) I think asymptomatic cases are a serious problem for us; (2) At some point in the future, we could all be threatened by asymptomatic cases; (3) I think asymptomatic cases have very serious consequences; (4) I think asymptomatic cases are a very serious problem; and (4) I think the presence of asymptomatic cases is a serious threat to my health (mean 5.26, SD 1.18; Cronbach α=0.89).Perceived Susceptibility
Perceived susceptibility was measured with two items taken from an existing scale [
]: (1) I feel that I am also at risk of being infected by asymptomatic cases and (2) I feel that I may also be infected by asymptomatic cases (mean 4.86, SD 1.76; Pearson r=0.83, P<.001).Information Seeking
Information seeking was measured based on the scale developed by Brashers et al [
] with the following eight items: (1) I would like to learn more information on asymptomatic cases than what is provided in this report; (2) I may discuss asymptomatic cases with people close to me (eg, friends, family); (3) I may ask my doctor about asymptomatic cases; (4) I may seek other news reports about asymptomatic cases; (5) I may pay close attention to news reports about asymptomatic cases that I encounter in the future; (6) I may check the internet for other information about asymptomatic cases; (7) I am likely to pay close attention to information on asymptomatic cases that I encounter on the internet in the future; and (8) I may read the scientific research literature on asymptomatic cases (mean 5.46, SD 1.30; Cronbach α=0.94).Information Avoidance
Information avoidance was measured using the scales developed by Afifi and Weiner [
] and Evans et al [ ] with the following six items: (1) I may try to change the subject if people close to me (eg, friends and family) discuss the issue of asymptomatic cases; (2) I may try to change the subject if my doctor discusses asymptomatic cases; (3) I may avoid exposure to other news reports about asymptomatic cases; (4) I may avoid exposure to information on the internet about asymptomatic cases; (5) I may try not to think too much about asymptomatic cases; and (6) I may try to forget about asymptomatic cases (mean 2.76, SD 1.73; Cronbach α=0.96).Willingness to Adopt Preventive Behaviors
Willingness to adopt preventive behaviors was measured via 10 items using a 7-point Likert scale (1=strongly agree to 7=strongly disagree), which measured respondents’ willingness to adopt a range of preventive behaviors such as strengthening protection, following health instructions, trying more preventive measures, and getting vaccinated. The specific items were: (1) I have decided to strengthen my preventive measures against COVID-19 immediately; (2) I intend to strengthen my protective measures against COVID-19 in the future; (3) I will pay more attention to asymptomatic cases; (4) I will persuade people around me to pay more attention to asymptomatic cases; (5) I will strictly follow professional health instructions to prevent catching the disease; (6) I will persuade people around me to strictly follow professional health instructions to prevent disease; (7) I will try as many ways as possible to prevent disease; (8) I will persuade those around me to try as many ways as possible to prevent disease; (9) I will get vaccinated as soon as a COVID-19 vaccine is developed; and (10) I will persuade those around me to get vaccinated as soon as a COVID-19 vaccine is developed (mean 5.54, SD 1.27; Cronbach α=0.89).
Vulnerability to False Information
Vulnerability to false information was measured by presenting respondents with 10 pieces of false news on COVID-19 (with five real news articles provided as distractors) to measure their trust in the false news. Sample items included the following:
Isatis root is suitable for treating conditions such as the common cold and viral influenza with heat symptoms. It has an antiviral effect and can help to prevent COVID-19.
Tobacco particles are at the nanometer scale and can evenly cover the surface of lung cells, forming a barrier that keeps viruses out of the body. Therefore, smoking can reduce the infection of the novel coronavirus.
The novel coronavirus is primarily a respiratory infection virus. Gargling with saline solution can eliminate the novel coronavirus bacteria that enter through the mouth.
These items were measured on a scale of 1 (strongly disbelieve) to 10 (strongly believe) (mean 3.68, SD 1.46; Cronbach α=0.89).
Data Analysis
To answer the four research questions, ANOVA was performed to examine the main and interaction effects of the manipulated independent variables (ie, uncertainty) on dependent outcomes (ie, risk perceptions and misinformation vulnerability). The mediation model was used as a posthoc analysis to estimate statistically significant differences between experimental conditions on outcomes. SPSS (version 28.0) and PROCESS (version 4.2) were used for all statistical analyses.
Results
Effects of Uncertainty Framing in News Coverage
According to the ANOVA results (
), there was a significant interaction effect of threat uncertainty and solution uncertainty on individuals’ vulnerability to misinformation (F1, 367=5.10, P=.02; partial η2=0.01). provides the complete data on group comparisons. Specifically, individuals who read news containing a certain threat and uncertain solution (mean 3.86, SD 0.15) were more likely to believe misinformation than those who read news containing a certain threat and certain solution (mean 3.47, SD 0.16). However, there was neither a significant main effect of threat uncertainty (partial η2=0.00) nor a significant main effect of solution uncertainty (partial η2=0.00) on individuals’ vulnerability to false information ( ).In terms of information processing (
), the threat uncertainty of health information showed a significant effect in promoting information-seeking behavior (partial η2=0.012). This implies that people who read news with the presence of an uncertain threat were more likely to search for additional relevant information than those who read news with absence of an uncertain threat. However, the uncertainty framing in the news, regardless of the type of uncertainty, did not affect individuals’ information avoidance behaviors.Effect | Severity | Susceptibility | Misinformation vulnerability | |
Threat | ||||
Certainty, mean (SD) | 5.12 (0.10) | 4.86 (0.13) | 3.67 (0.11) | |
Uncertainty, mean (SD) | 5.34 (0.10) | 4.86 (0.13) | 3.68 (0.11) | |
F (df=1, 367) | 1.68 | 0.00 | 0.01 | |
P value | .20 | >.99 | .91 | |
Solution | ||||
Certainty, mean (SD) | 5.10 (0.10) | 4.78 (0.13) | 3.65 (0.11) | |
Uncertainty, mean (SD) | 5.39 (0.10) | 4.94 (0.13) | 3.70 (0.10) | |
F (df=1, 367) | 4.29 | 0.78 | 0.09 | |
P value | .04 | .38 | .76 |
Effect | Information seeking | Information avoidance | |||
Threat | |||||
Certainty, mean (SD) | 5.31 (0.10) | 2.89 (0.13) | |||
Uncertainty, mean (SD) | 5.60 (0.10) | 2.62 (0.13) | |||
F (df=1, 367) | 4.47 | 2.26 | |||
P value | .04 | .13 | |||
Solution | |||||
Certainty, mean (SD) | 5.40 (0.10) | 2.60 (0.13) | |||
Uncertainty, mean (SD) | 5.51 (0.10) | 2.91 (0.12) | |||
F (df=1, 367) | 0.61 | 3.07 | |||
P value | .43 | .08 |
Sequential Mediation Analysis
Serial linear regression with PROCESS macro model 81 was used to analyze how the dummy-coded variables (1=uncertain threat and certain solution, 2=certain threat and uncertain solution, 3=uncertain threat and uncertain solution, and reference (0)=certain threat and certain solution) influence preventive behavioral intention through the factors perceived severity, information seeking, and information avoidance. A significant serial mediation model was detected, in which the perceived severity and information seeking would sequentially mediate the relationship between the exposure to news containing a certain threat and an uncertain solution (point estimate 0.17, SE 0.08; 95% CI 0.02-0.33) or news containing an uncertain threat and an uncertain solution (point estimate 0.18, SE 0.08; 95% CI 0.03-0.34) and the protective behavioral intention (see
for the path significance and coefficients).The mediation models obtained with PROCESS macro model 81, including misinformation vulnerability, information seeking, and information avoidance as the three mediators, were established with the same three dummy-coded comparison variables described above. Although the serial mediating effect of misinformation vulnerability and information processing on the relationship was nonsignificant, significant associations between misinformation vulnerability on information processing and protective behavioral intention were detected (see
for the path significance and coefficients).Discussion
Principal Findings
This study explored the impact of incorporating different levels of uncertainty in news articles on individuals’ risk perception, susceptibility to misinformation, subsequent information processing, and intention toward preventive behaviors. These factors play a crucial role in guiding individuals to protect themselves during a public health crisis.
First, as expected, the findings showed that individuals would perceive the crisis to be of greater severity when reading news framed withing a context of solution uncertainty, regardless of whether or not the threat of the crisis was framed as certain or uncertain in the news article. Furthermore, the perceived severity would in turn motivate individuals’ information-seeking and information-avoidance behaviors. As suggested by Gudykunst [
], the experience of fear and concern is also associated with uncertainty. Individuals’ fear might be evoked when they fail to find a solution to solve the risky problem, which would consequently increase their perception of the severity of the health threat. Meanwhile, to cope with the fear, individuals would either seek more information or avoid more information [ ].Second, individuals tended to believe false information during a public health crisis, especially after reading a news article containing certain threats and uncertain solutions. Furthermore, posthoc regression analysis suggested that both information seeking and information avoidance were positively associated with misinformation vulnerability. As expected, information seeking was positively associated with protective behavioral intentions, while information avoidance was negatively associated with protective behavioral intention. That said, communicating crisis uncertainty in a news article would be risky in terms of increasing the public’s vulnerability to misinformation. Moreover, misinformation vulnerability would further motivate information avoidance, which would consequently dissuade individuals’ intention to adopt preventive behaviors. However, communicating crisis uncertainty could also be beneficial because the increased misinformation vulnerability that arises after reading news with related uncertainty could simultaneously motivate both expected health information–seeking and protective behaviors.
Third, our findings suggest that compared to an uncertain threat, uncertain solutions are more potentially problematic. An uncertain solution with either an uncertain or certain threat had a greater impact on individuals’ perceived severity of the crisis. Moreover, an uncertain solution with an uncertain threat led to higher vulnerability to misinformation. Although both perceived severity and misinformation vulnerability could motivate expected health information–seeking or protective behavioral intentions, they were likely to trigger information avoidance, which would further impair the protective behavioral intention. Given these conflicting findings, more research is needed to understand the mechanism behind the effects of communicating solution uncertainty, especially during a public health crisis.
Implications and Limitations
A few limitations of this study should be noted to provide inspiring suggestions for further research. First, the generalizability of the findings needs to be addressed. This study is based on an exceptionally unique context of a public health event. In April 2020, the ambiguity surrounding the perceived risk of COVID-19 and the appropriate preventive measures was prevalent. Different regions in China implemented varying degrees of epidemic control measures, leading to divergent strategies. Media coverage of “asymptomatic carriers” and other aspects of the COVID-19 pandemic often exhibited inconsistent or contradictory information. Thus, the diverse experimental scenarios were based on natural contexts for presenting four distinct threat-and-solution scenarios and would not induce a perceptual conflict for the participants. However, when extrapolating to other research topics, it is imperative to consider the coherence between experimental scenarios and real-world settings.
Second, the impact of the research topic on participants with different characteristics requires further discussion. This study endeavored to achieve a balanced representation of participants with respect to sex, education level, and income bracket. Consequently, an exhaustive examination of the differential effects stemming from various sociodemographic factors on the outcomes was not performed. However, in distinct research inquiries, this divergence may prove consequential. Hence, future research endeavors could delve deeper into understanding the perception of information uncertainty and management behaviors across populations with diverse backgrounds and characteristics.
Third, although this type of experimental design typically includes 80-90 participants per group to achieve sufficient statistical power, the unexpected smaller effect size in this study resulted in a reduced statistical power of approximately 60%. This diminutive effect size may compromise the study’s sensitivity in detecting meaningful relationships, thereby affecting the reliability and generalizability of the findings. To enhance statistical power and ensure robust results in similar experimental studies in China, a larger sample size is recommended.
Regarding the implications, this study aimed to elucidate the impact of health information uncertainty on individuals’ information-processing mechanisms. The results thus provide further evidence for the impacts of individuals’ perceptions and behaviors underlying uncertainty management theory. Amid public health events inundated with uncertain information, individuals’ perceptions and behaviors related to uncertainty management often determine their attitudes toward addressing health threats and the potential adoption of health measures. Therefore, comprehending this process contributes to facilitating more effective health communication between the public health system and the general public. Faced with uncertain public health events, participants such as public health institutions, media, and the general public should all take into account the implications of information uncertainty, ensuring the effective dissemination of information throughout all stages of the crisis.
Acknowledgments
This work was supported by the National Social Science Fund of China (project name: Construction and Impact Research of Trust Mechanisms in Health Communication during the “Information Epidemic”; project number 22CXW022).
Conflicts of Interest
None declared.
Simulated news articles presented to the participants in the online experiment under four conditions varying in threat and solution uncertainty about the COVID-19 pandemic.
DOCX File , 1829 KBReferences
- Capurro G, Jardine CG, Tustin J, Driedger M. Communicating scientific uncertainty in a rapidly evolving situation: a framing analysis of Canadian coverage in early days of COVID-19. BMC Public Health. Nov 29, 2021;21(1):2181. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Büchter RB, Betsch C, Ehrlich M, Fechtelpeter D, Grouven U, Keller S, et al. Communicating uncertainty in written consumer health information to the public: parallel-group, web-based randomized controlled trial. J Med Internet Res. Aug 10, 2020;22(8):e15899. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- van den Bos K, Ham J, Lind EA, Simonis M, van Essen WJ, Rijpkema M. Justice and the human alarm system: the impact of exclamation points and flashing lights on the justice judgment process. J Exp Soc Psychol. Mar 2008;44(2):201-219. [CrossRef]
- Afifi WA, Weiner JL. Toward a theory of motivated information management. Commun Theory. May 2004;14(2):167-190. [CrossRef]
- Chu TH, Yeo TED, Su Y. Effects of exposure to COVID-19 news and information: a meta-analysis of media use and uncertainty-related responses during the pandemic. Journalism Mass Commun Quart. Jan 04, 2022;99(1):89-112. [CrossRef]
- Bed bugs FAQs. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. URL: https://www.cdc.gov/parasites/bedbugs/faqs.html [accessed 2020-09-16]
- So J, Kuang K, Cho H. Information seeking upon exposure to risk messages: predictors, outcomes, and mediating roles of health information seeking. Commun Res. Nov 24, 2016;46(5):663-687. [CrossRef]
- Witte K, Cameron KA, McKeon JK, Berkowitz JM. Predicting risk behaviors: development and validation of a diagnostic scale. J Health Commun. Oct 29, 1996;1(4):317-341. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Lalot F, Abrams D, Travaglino GA. Aversion amplification in the emerging COVID‐19 pandemic: the impact of political trust and subjective uncertainty on perceived threat. Commun Appl Soc Psy. Dec 13, 2020;31(2):213-222. [CrossRef]
- Pine KH, Lee M, Whitman SA, Chen Y, Henne K. Making sense of risk information amidst uncertainty: individuals' perceived risks associated with the COVID-19 pandemic. 2021. Presented at: CHI '21: CHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems; May 8-13, 2021; Yokohama, Japan. [CrossRef]
- Lu J, Zhang M, Zheng Y, Li Q. Communication of uncertainty about preliminary evidence and the spread of its inferred misinformation during the COVID-19 pandemic-a Weibo case study. Int J Environ Res Public Health. Nov 13, 2021;18(22):11933. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Zhao X, Fan J, Basnyat I, Hu B. Online health information seeking using "#COVID-19 Patient Seeking Help" on Weibo in Wuhan, China: descriptive study. J Med Internet Res. Oct 15, 2020;22(10):e22910. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Afifi WA, Weiner JL. Seeking information about sexual health: applying the theory of motivated information management. Human Comm Res. Jan 2006;32(1):35-57. [CrossRef]
- Evans BM, Kairam S, Pirolli P. Do your friends make you smarter?: an analysis of social strategies in online information seeking. Inf Process Manage. Nov 2010;46(6):679-692. [CrossRef]
- Hogan TP, Brashers DE. The theory of communication and uncertainty management: implications from the wider realm of information behavior. In: Afifi T, Afifi W, editors. Uncertainty, Information Management, and Disclosure Decisions: Theories and Applications. New York. Routledge/Taylor & Francis Group; 2009;45-66.
- Charpentier CJ, Cogliati Dezza I, Vellani V, Globig LK, Gädeke M, Sharot T. Anxiety increases information-seeking in response to large changes. Sci Rep. May 05, 2022;12(1):7385. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Keller AM, Taylor HA, Brunyé TT. Uncertainty promotes information-seeking actions, but what information? Cogn Res Princ Implic. Sep 07, 2020;5(1):42. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Brainin E, Neter E. Refined analysis of older eHealth users from an agency perspective: quantitative telephone interview study. JMIR Aging. Apr 26, 2023;6:e40004. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Case DO, Andrews JE, Johnson JD, Allard SL. Avoiding versus seeking: the relationship of information seeking to avoidance, blunting, coping, dissonance, and related concepts. J Med Libr Assoc. Jul 2005;93(3):353-362. [FREE Full text] [Medline]
- Goodall CE, Reed P. Threat and efficacy uncertainty in news coverage about bed bugs as unique predictors of information seeking and avoidance: an extension of the EPPM. Health Commun. Jan 2013;28(1):63-71. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Jiang S, Liu PL, Ngien A, Wu Q. The effects of worry, risk perception, information-seeking experience, and trust in misinformation on COVID-19 fact-checking: a survey study in China. Chinese J Commun. Sep 12, 2022;16(2):132-149. [CrossRef]
- Liu BF, Viens J. Crisis and risk communication scholarship of the future: reflections on research gaps. J Int Crisis Ris Commun Res. Mar 9, 2020;3(1):7-14. [CrossRef]
- Liu BF, Xu S, Rhys Lim J, Egnoto M. How publics’ active and passive communicative behaviors affect their tornado responses: an integration of STOPS and SMCC. Public Relat Rev. Nov 2019;45(4):101831. [CrossRef]
- Griffin RJ, Dunwoody S, Neuwirth K. Proposed model of the relationship of risk information seeking and processing to the development of preventive behaviors. Environ Res. Feb 1999;80(2 Pt 2):S230-S245. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Bode L, Vraga EK. See something, say something: correction of global health misinformation on social media. Health Commun. Sep 2018;33(9):1131-1140. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Tan ASL, Lee C, Chae J. Exposure to health (mis)information: lagged effects on young adults' health behaviors and potential pathways. J Commun. Jul 06, 2015;65(4):674-698. [CrossRef]
- Soroya SH, Farooq A, Mahmood K, Isoaho J, Zara S. From information seeking to information avoidance: understanding the health information behavior during a global health crisis. Inf Process Manag. Mar 2021;58(2):102440. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Sharit J, Czaja SJ. Overcoming older adult barriers to learning through an understanding of perspectives on human information processing. J Appl Gerontol. Mar 30, 2020;39(3):233-241. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Dai B, Ali A, Wang H. Exploring information avoidance intention of social media users: a cognition–affect–conation perspective. Internet Res. Jul 14, 2020;30(5):1455-1478. [CrossRef]
- "Asymptomatic infections" enter daily bulletin: epidemic prevention and control must be realistic and pragmatic. Xinhua News Agency. URL: http://www.xinhuanet.com/politics/2020-04/02/c_1125803965.htm [accessed 2020-04-02]
- Brashers DE, Goldsmith DJ, Hsieh E. Information seeking and avoiding in health contexts. Human Comm Res. Apr 2002;28(2):258-271. [CrossRef]
- Gudykunst W. Anxiety/uncertainty management (AUM) theory: current status. In: Wiseman R, editor. Intercultural Communication Theory. New York. Sage Publications; 1995;8-58.
Edited by G Eysenbach, L Beri; submitted 16.08.23; peer-reviewed by S Su, X Xu, A Chang; comments to author 06.12.23; revised version received 08.01.24; accepted 12.04.24; published 14.05.24.
Copyright©Rui Zhao, Xuerong Lu, Jiayi Yang, Biao Li. Originally published in the Journal of Medical Internet Research (https://www.jmir.org), 14.05.2024.
This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work, first published in the Journal of Medical Internet Research, is properly cited. The complete bibliographic information, a link to the original publication on https://www.jmir.org/, as well as this copyright and license information must be included.