Published on in Vol 25 (2023)

Preprints (earlier versions) of this paper are available at https://preprints.jmir.org/preprint/41082, first published .
Turning When Using Smartphone in Persons With and Those Without Neurologic Conditions: Observational Study

Turning When Using Smartphone in Persons With and Those Without Neurologic Conditions: Observational Study

Turning When Using Smartphone in Persons With and Those Without Neurologic Conditions: Observational Study

Original Paper

1Department of Neurology, Kiel University, Kiel, Germany

2Department of Neuroscience, Mental Health and Sensory Organs (NESMOS), Sapienza University of Rome, Rome, Italy

3Division of Surgery, Saarland University, Homburg, Germany

4Faculty of Engineering, Kiel University, Kiel, Germany

5Institute of Medical Informatics and Statistics, University Hospital Schleswig-Holstein, Kiel University, Kiel, Germany

6Santa Lucia Foundation, Rome, Italy

Corresponding Author:

Walter Maetzler, MD

Department of Neurology

Kiel University

Klinik für Neurologie UKSH, Campus Kiel

Arnold-Heller-Strasse 3, Haus D

Kiel, 24105

Germany

Phone: 49 431500 23981

Fax:49 431500 23848

Email: w.maetzler@neurologie.uni-kiel.de


Background: Turning during walking is a relevant and common everyday movement and it depends on a correct top-down intersegmental coordination. This could be reduced in several conditions (en bloc turning), and an altered turning kinematics has been linked to increased risk of falls. Smartphone use has been associated with poorer balance and gait; however, its effect on turning-while-walking has not been investigated yet. This study explores turning intersegmental coordination during smartphone use in different age groups and neurologic conditions.

Objective: This study aims to evaluate the effect of smartphone use on turning behavior in healthy individuals of different ages and those with various neurological diseases.

Methods: Younger (aged 18-60 years) and older (aged >60 years) healthy individuals and those with Parkinson disease, multiple sclerosis, subacute stroke (<4 weeks), or lower-back pain performed turning-while-walking alone (single task [ST]) and while performing 2 different cognitive tasks of increasing complexity (dual task [DT]). The mobility task consisted of walking up and down a 5-m walkway at self-selected speed, thus including 180° turns. Cognitive tasks consisted of a simple reaction time test (simple DT [SDT]) and a numerical Stroop test (complex DT [CDT]). General (turn duration and the number of steps while turning), segmental (peak angular velocity), and intersegmental turning parameters (intersegmental turning onset latency and maximum intersegmental angle) were extracted for head, sternum, and pelvis using a motion capture system and a turning detection algorithm.

Results: In total, 121 participants were enrolled. All participants, irrespective of age and neurologic disease, showed a reduced intersegmental turning onset latency and a reduced maximum intersegmental angle of both pelvis and sternum relative to head, thus indicating an en bloc turning behavior when using a smartphone. With regard to change from the ST to turning when using a smartphone, participants with Parkinson disease reduced their peak angular velocity the most, which was significantly different from lower-back pain relative to the head (P<.01). Participants with stroke showed en bloc turning already without smartphone use.

Conclusions: Smartphone use during turning-while-walking may lead to en bloc turning and thus increase fall risk across age and neurologic disease groups. This behavior is probably particularly dangerous for those groups with the most pronounced changes in turning parameters during smartphone use and the highest fall risk, such as individuals with Parkinson disease. Moreover, the experimental paradigm presented here might be useful in differentiating individuals with lower-back pain without and those with early or prodromal Parkinson disease. In individuals with subacute stroke, en bloc turning could represent a compensative strategy to overcome the newly occurring mobility deficit. Considering the ubiquitous smartphone use in daily life, this study should stimulate future studies in the area of fall risk and neurological and orthopedic diseases.

Trial Registration: German Clinical Trials Register DRKS00022998; https://drks.de/search/en/trial/DRKS00022998

J Med Internet Res 2023;25:e41082

doi:10.2196/41082

Keywords



Turning-while-walking is relevant to everyday life and very common, accounting for >40% of steps taken in daily activities [1]. The movement itself requires a good coordination of body segments and control of body rotation toward the new direction while maintaining dynamic stability. In healthy adults, a top-down temporal sequence in body segments reorientation around the vertical axis, while turning has been described [2-5]. This starts with eyes and head movement to fix the gaze onto the new direction, followed by sternum, pelvis, and finally feet rotation. Being a challenging task, turning renders individuals at higher risk of falling, particularly in older adults and people who have mobility impairments such as neurologic patients [6,7].

Previous evidence supports the negative association of an effective top-down sequence of body segments reorientation with the risk of falls, with people reorientating body segments more simultaneously being more prone to have multiple falls [8]. This strategy is generally called en bloc turning. On these premises, some studies investigated the influence of kinematic and physiological factors and pathological conditions on turning behavior. An influence of kinematic factors such as turning velocity, on timing and magnitude of segmental reorientation, has been suggested; however, only a few studies have investigated these elements, with conflicting results [5,9,10]. Age also seems to have an influence on intersegmental turning behavior with the head, sternum, and pelvis turning more simultaneously in older subjects [9,11]. Considering the neurologic disorders, the results are often inconsistent with some studies showing a preserved top-down reorientation in chronic stroke [12-16] and individuals with Parkinson disease [17,18], while others studies reported en bloc turning behavior for these individuals [3,19-23]. This could depend, at least partially, on the different experimental protocols (eg, different turning magnitudes, on-the-spot turning, and turning-while-walking), enrolled populations, and measures used to investigate turning behavior (eg, number and selection of body segments, temporal and spatial measures). Only a few studies have investigated turning intersegmental coordination in individuals with lower-back pain [24,25] and focused only on the sternum and pelvis, while no studies, to the best of our knowledge, investigated turning intersegmental coordination in individuals with multiple sclerosis.

Turning is particularly dangerous when performed during multitasking. This term refers to the performance of multiple tasks simultaneously, such as walking and talking or driving while speaking at the phone. Multitasking is very common in everyday life, and several activities are performed while walking and turning. Performing a secondary task while walking (dual task [DT]) could negatively affect gait performance [26] and predict future falls [27], and several theories have been proposed to explain this effect, such as limited attention allocation capacity, a bottleneck effect for tasks performed in parallel, and competition for computational resources [28]. Differently from gait, only a few studies investigated the effect of DT on turning behavior. Most studies focused on general turning parameters such as the number of steps taken, turn duration, or peak angular velocity in both healthy subjects [29,30] and individuals with neurologic conditions [30-33]. Only 1 study, to the best of our knowledge, investigated the effect of DT on intersegmental coordination [23], showing a disruption of turning sequence in individuals who have chronic stroke only under cognitive DT condition, while healthy participants showed a preserved top-down sequence.

Similarly, no study to date has investigated the effect of smartphone use on turning behavior. This is a ubiquitous condition in everyday life, and in the last decade, some evidence has shown a detrimental effect of smartphone use on straight walking and balance [34-37].

To our best knowledge, this is the first study evaluating the effect of smartphone use on turning behavior in healthy participants of different ages and those with various neurological diseases.


Population

Participants were recruited through flyers placed in public facilities (healthy participants) and in the Department of Neurology and outpatient clinics at University Hospital Schleswig-Holstein, Campus Kiel, Germany (neurological patients). Inclusion criteria were (1) being aged 18 years and older and (2) ability to walk independently without walking aids. Exclusion criteria were (1) a Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) score of <15 and (2) other movement impairments affecting mobility performance, as judged by the assessor. Participants were divided into 6 groups according to age and diagnosis. Healthy participants were divided into “young” (aged 18-60 years) and “older” (aged > 60 years). Participants with neurological disorders included individuals with Parkinson disease (according to the UK Brain Bank criteria [38]), multiple sclerosis (according to McDonalds criteria [39]), subacute stroke (<4 weeks), or lower-back pain as indicated on the basis of the patients’ medical history and diagnosis upon examination [40].

Ethics Approval

The study was approved by the ethical committee of the Medical Faculty of Kiel University (D438/18) and was conducted in accordance with the principles of the Declaration of Helsinki. All participants provided written informed consent before the start of measurements. The study is registered in the German Clinical Trials Register (DRKS00022998, registration date: September 4, 2020).

Demographic and Clinical Data

Demographic data including age, sex, weight, and height were collected. Overall cognitive function at baseline was assessed with the MoCA [41]. Participants' mobility was assessed with the Short Physical Performance Battery [42]. Disease-specific evaluation included the Movement Disorder Society Unified Parkinson's Disease Rating Scale part III [43] and the Hoehn and Yahr Scale [44] for participants with Parkinson disease (performed in ON condition); the Expanded Disability Status Scale [45] for participants with multiple sclerosis; the National Institute of Health Stroke Scale [46] for participants with subacute stroke, and a visual analog scale of pain intensity [47], and the German Funktionsfragenbogen Hannover Scale [48] for participants with lower-back pain.

Experimental Procedure

Participants were asked to perform an overground walking and turning task on a 5-m-long walkway with a width of 1 m. Participants were asked to walk up and down the walkway at self-selected speed, thus including 180° turns to change walking direction (Figure 1). Walking and turning tasks were performed as a single task (ST) for a duration of 30 seconds, and while performing 2 different cognitive tasks of similar duration (~30 seconds) of increasing complexity: a simple reaction time test (simple DT [SDT]) and a numerical Stroop test (complex DT [CDT]). Both cognitive tasks were administered via a handheld smartphone. ST, SDT, and CDT conditions were performed consecutively. In SDT, participants had to tap the screen as fast as possible after the appearance of a black square. In CDT, participants had to choose the higher number from 2 options [49]. The Stroop test was administered under 3 conditions: (1) congruent, in which the number with a higher value had a larger character size; (2) neutral, in which the character size of both numbers was equal; (3) incongruent, in which the number with a higher value had a smaller character size. Participants performed a practice trial on both ST and DT conditions to familiarize themselves with the procedure. More details on the study protocol can be found elsewhere [50].

Figure 1. Schematic representation of the 5-m walkway and the turning-while walking task.

Movement and Rotation Analysis

A 12-camera 3D optical motion capture system (Qualisys AB) was used to record marker trajectories of passive retroreflective markers attached on the skin or tight clothing and footwear of the participants. Reflective markers were placed, among others, on the head, sternum, pelvis, and left and right feet. The exact placement of the markers is described elsewhere [50]. Data were recorded at 200 Hz. The orientation of the head, sternum, and pelvis was calculated based on the reflective markers on each of these segments. To detect turns during walking, angles of the head, sternum, and pelvis were calculated according to Euler method using the orientation of the segments derived from the position of the reflective markers [51]. The start and end of a turn were detected by the change in SD. A change in SD from the mean indicates an abrupt change in the signal. At the start and end of a turn, an abrupt change in the angular signal around the vertical axis can be seen. The detection of this abrupt change in signal was carried out with the MATLAB function findchangepts. This function detects the point at which a mean changes most significantly by partitioning data into 2 regions that minimize the sum of the residual (squared) error of each region from its local mean. Steps were detected from the heel and toe marker trajectories. For this purpose, vertical velocity signals were computed from the heel and toe markers’ position data [52], and initial contact was determined from a local minimum in the vertical velocity of the toe marker, whereas final contact was determined from a local maximum in the vertical velocity of the heel marker [53].

Measures

For the analysis of turning behavior, we calculated general turning parameters as well as segmental and intersegmental measures for the head, sternum, and pelvis. Regarding general parameters, turn duration, defined as the time in seconds between the beginning and ending of the turning phase, and the number of steps taken while turning were calculated. Concerning segmental turning measures, the peak angular velocity for each of the 3 body segments was measured. Finally, to investigate intersegmental coordination during turning in the temporal and spatial domains, we calculated intersegmental relative turning onset latencies and intersegmental maximum angles, respectively. These were calculated for each pair of segments for a total of 3 pairs. Turning onset latencies were determined using the more cranial one as a reference in each pair (sternum relative to the head, pelvis relative to the head, and pelvis relative to the sternum). A negative value indicates that the cranial segment started turning first. For each participant, variables were calculated as the average across turns for each of the 3 conditions.

Data and Statistical Analysis

To evaluate the impact of DT on turn duration, number of steps, and peak angular velocity for the 3 body segments, dual-task cost (DTC) was calculated for both SDT and CDT on the basis of equation 1 [54,55]:

Statistical analyses were performed using JASP (version 0.16.1; JASP Team), R (version 4.0.3; The R Foundation), and RStudio (version 2022.02.2+433 for Windows; R Foundation for Statistical Computing). Descriptive statistics were calculated for the examined variables. To assess the difference in turn duration, number of steps, peak angular velocity, turning onset latencies, and maximum angles across the different groups and different conditions, mixed ANOVAs, corrected for gait speed, were used, with the factors “condition” and “group.” Post hoc t tests with Bonferroni correction were performed in case of significant ANOVA main effects. To assess the difference in DTC for turn duration, number of steps, and peak angular velocity between the different groups and between SDT and CDT, mixed ANOVAs, corrected for gait speed, were used, with the factors “DT condition” and “group.” To evaluate the segmental sequence of body segment while turning, mixed ANOVAs, corrected for gait speed, with the factors “segment pair turning onset latency” and “group” were used. If necessary, Greenhouse-Geisser correction for nonsphericity were applied. The significance threshold was set at α<.05. All data were reported as mean (SD) or median (IQR) for numerical data and N (%) for categorical variables.


A total of 121 participants were enrolled in the study. Table 1 shows the demographic and clinical data of the included groups. Average turn magnitudes detected by our algorithm for head, sternum, and pelvis were 160° (SD 8°), 153° (SD 7°), and 155° (SD 7°), respectively. An example of algorithm output is shown in Figure 2.

Table 1. Demographics and clinical data of the enrolled groups.

Young adults (N=36)Older adults (N=18)Participants with Parkinson disease (N=26)Participants with subacute stroke (N=14)Participants with multiple sclerosis (N=19)Participants with lower-back pain (N=8)
Age (years), mean (SD)28.9 (8.3)71.9 (6.3)63.3 (10.9)63.7 (16.6)38.5 (13.0)63.6 (16.8)
Sex, n (%)

Male15 (42)9 (50)10 (38)3 (21)11 (58)3 (37)

Female21 (58)9 (50)16 (62)11 (79)8 (42)5 (63)
Height (cm), mean (SD)180 (9)173 (10)175 (8)175 (11)179 (11)174 (10)
Weight (kg), mean (SD)74.8 (14)78.4 (16.3)83.4 (15.2)81.4 (16.5)81.7 (21.2)79.8 (18.1)
BMI, mean (SD)22.9 (3)26.1 (5.1)27.1 (4)26.4 (4.8)25.3 (4.7)26.3 (3.6)
MoCAa score, median (IQR)29 (28-30)23.5 (21-27)25 (23-26)23 (20-26)28 (26-29)25 (24-27)
SPPBb score, median (IQR)12 (12-12)11 (9-11)10 (8-11)11 (9-11)9 (7-11)12 (8-12)
H&Yc score, median (IQR)d2 (1-3)
MDS-UPDRS-IIIe score, median (IQR)22 (12-28)
EDSSf score, median (IQR)1 (1-4)
NIHSSg score, median (IQR)0 (0-3)
pVASh score, median (IQR)3 (0-6)4 (1-6)
FFbHi score, median (IQR)30 (27-32)3 (29-31)

aMoCA: Montreal Cognitive Assessment.

bSPPB: Short Physical Performance Battery.

cH&Y: Hoehn and Yahr Scale.

dNot determined.

eMDS-UPDRS-III: Movement Disorder Society Unified Parkinson\'s Disease Rating Scale part III.

fEDSS: Expanded Disability Status Scale.

gNIHSS: National Institute of Health Stroke Scale.

hpVAS: visual analog scale of pain intensity.

iFFbH: Funktionsfragenbogen Hannover Scale.

Figure 2. Example of the turning detection algorithm output. Angular signals of the pelvis, sternum, and head are reported. The vertical lines indicate the start and end of a turn for each body segment.

Turning Sequence

Significant effects of factor “group” (F5,99=2.689; P=.03) and “segment pair turning onset latency” (F1.51,149.03=104.5; P<.001) but no significant interaction between the 2 factors were found in ST condition. No significant effects of the 2 factors, or interaction between the 2 factors, were found for the SDT or the CDT condition. Considering factor “segment pair,” post hoc analysis showed a significant difference between turning onset latency of the pelvis relative to the sternum and both turning onset latency of the sternum relative to the head and the pelvis relative to the head in all groups (all P<.01), except in participants with subacute stroke.

Turn Duration and Number of Steps

Significant effects of factor “group” and “condition,” but no significant interaction between the 2 factors were found for turn duration (F5,83=8.091, P<.001; F1.40,116.42=18.269, P<.001) and number of steps while turning (F5,78=2.988, P=.02; F1.45,112.79=12.4, P<.001).

Peak Angular Velocity

Significant effects of factor “group” and “condition” were found for peak angular velocity of the head (F5,99=12.878, P<.001; F1.57,155.86=112.506, P<.001), sternum (F5,95=8.569, P<.001; F1.51,146.67=4.204, P=.03), and pelvis (F5,94=10.171, P<.001; F1.76,165.47=24.844, P<.001). A significant interaction between the 2 factors was found only for peak angular velocity of the pelvis (F8.80,165.47=2.520; P=.01).

Intersegmental Relative Turning Onset Latencies

Significant effects of factor “condition” were found for both turning onset latency of the sternum relative to the head (F1.39,120.82=116.121; P<.001) and the pelvis relative to the head (F1.15,99.75=66.019; P<.001). A significant effect of factor “group” and interaction between the 2 factors was found only for turning onset latency of the sternum relative to the head (F5,87=2.683, P=.03; F6.94,120.82=2.109; P=.048). No significant effects or interaction between the 2 factors were found for turning onset latency of the pelvis relative to the sternum. Details of post hoc comparisons can be found in Figures 3-5.

Figure 3. Turning onset latencies by group (A) and task condition (B) of the sternum relative to the head. Mean of the variables is represented by bar height. SE of the mean is shown by the vertical lines. A negative value means that the cranial body segment starts turning first and vice versa. Significant pairwise comparisons are marked by horizontal lines and asterisks as follows: *P<.05; **P<.01; ***P<.001. CDT: complex dual task; LBP: Lower-back pain; MS: multiple sclerosis; PD: Parkinson disease; SDT: simple dual task; ST: single task.
Figure 4. Turning onset latencies by group (A) and task condition (B) of the pelvis relative to the head. Mean of the variables is represented by bar height. SE of the mean is shown by the vertical lines. A negative value means that the cranial body segment starts turning first and vice-versa. Significant pairwise comparisons are marked by horizontal lines and asterisks as follows: *P<.05; **P<.01; ***P<.001. CDT: complex dual task; LBP: Lower-back pain; MS: multiple sclerosis; PD: Parkinson disease; SDT: simple dual task; ST: single task.
Figure 5. Turning onset latencies by group (A) and task condition (B) of the pelvis relative to the sternum. Mean of the variables is represented by bar height. SE of the mean is shown by the vertical lines. A negative value means that the cranial body segment starts turning first and vice versa. Significant pairwise comparisons are marked by horizontal lines and asterisks as follows: *P<.05; **P<.01; ***P<.001. CDT: complex dual task; LBP: Lower-back pain; MS: multiple sclerosis; PD: Parkinson disease; SDT: simple dual task; ST: single task.

Intersegmental Maximum Angles

Significant effects of factor “condition” were found for maximum angle between the head and sternum (F1.30,128.97=104.478; P<.001), head and pelvis (F1.24,115.65=93.328; P<.001), and the sternum and pelvis (F1.73,164.21=4.660; P=.01) but not for factor “group” or interaction between the 2 factors for each of the 3 segments pairs. Details of post hoc comparisons can be found in Figures 6-8.

Figure 6. Maximum intersegmental angles by group (A) and task condition (B) between sternum and head. Mean of the variables is represented by bar height. SE of the mean is shown by the vertical lines. Significant pairwise comparisons are marked by horizontal lines and asterisks as follows: *P<.05; **P<.01; ***P<.001. CDT: complex dual task; LBP: Lower-back pain; MS: multiple sclerosis; PD: Parkinson disease; SDT: simple dual task; ST: single task.
Figure 7. Maximum intersegmental angles by group (A) and task condition (B) between pelvis and head. Mean of the variables is represented by bar height. SE of the mean is shown by the vertical lines. Significant pairwise comparisons are marked by horizontal lines and asterisks as follows: *P<.05; **P<.01; ***P<.001.CDT: complex dual task; LBP: Lower-back pain; MS: multiple sclerosis; PD: Parkinson disease; SDT: simple dual task; ST: single task.
Figure 8. Maximum intersegmental angles by group (A) and task condition (B) between pelvis and sternum. The mean of the variables is represented by bar height. SE of the mean is shown by the vertical lines. Significant pairwise comparisons are marked by horizontal lines and asterisks as follows: *P<.05; **P<.01; ***P<.001. CDT: complex dual task; LBP: Lower-back pain; MS: multiple sclerosis; PD: Parkinson disease; SDT: simple dual task; ST: single task.

Dual-Task Cost

A significant effect of factor “DT condition” (F1,70=7.216; P=.009) but not for factor “group” or interaction between the 2 factors was found for turn duration DTC. No significant effects or interactions between the 2 factors were found for DTC of number of steps while turning. Significant effects of factor “group” were found for DTC of peak angular velocity for head (F5,91=3.701; P=.004) and sternum (F5,72=3.2.382; P=.047). A significant effect of factor “DT condition” was found for DTC of peak angular velocity for the head (F5,91=7.028; P=.009) and pelvis (F1,82=8.237; P=.005). No interaction between the 2 factors was found for all 3 body segments. Post hoc analysis showed a significant difference between participants with Parkinson disease and those with lower-back pain for DTC of peak angular velocity for the head in both SDT and CDT conditions with participants with Parkinson disease, demonstrating a higher DTC (mean difference 33%, SD 9% and 32%, SD 10%, respectively; Figure 9).

More details regarding the pairwise comparisons for the investigated variables can be found in the Multimedia Appendix 1.

Figure 9. Dual task cost of peak angular velocity of the head by group (A) and task condition (B). The mean of the variables is represented by bar height. SE of the mean is shown by the vertical lines. Significant pairwise comparisons are marked by horizontal lines and asterisks as follows: *P<.05; **P<.01; ***P<.001. CDT: complex dual task; DT: dual task; DTC: dual-task cost; LBP: Lower-back pain; MS: multiple sclerosis; PD: Parkinson disease; SDT: simple dual task; ST: single task.

Overview

This cross-sectional study aimed at evaluating the effects of concurrent active smartphone usage and turning-while-walking on the dynamics of intersegmental coordination of turning. Both turning and smartphone use during walking are common in human life, and reduced turning coordination could lead to an increased risk of falling [8,56]. Smartphone use has already been associated with poorer balance and gait [35-37], but no study to date investigated the effect of smartphone use on turning behavior. Moreover, advanced age as well as several neurologic conditions are known to be associated with an increased risk of falling, and smartphone usage during turning might be particularly critical for specific groups. To determine the effects of age and neurologic conditions, both young and older participants as well as those with different neurologic disorders were included into the study.

Principal Results

We found that all participants, irrespective of age and neurologic disease, showed an en bloc turning behavior when using a smartphone; participants with Parkinson disease showed the most pronounced reduction in peak angular velocity, with a significant difference for the DTC of head segment compared to lower-back pain; and that our participants with subacute stroke turned en bloc even without a smartphone. These results are discussed in detail in the following paragraphs.

All groups turned en bloc while performing a cognitive task on a smartphone irrespective of age and neurologic condition. Although the direct effect of smartphone use on tuning has not been explored to date, reports investigating the influence of smartphone on straight walking and balance showed a reduction in gait speed [35,36,57], and altered dynamic and static balance [37]. This was linked to the cognitive load of smartphone use but also to the need to reduce and smoothen head movements to maintain gaze fixation onto the screen [57]. The increased turn duration and the reduction of head peak angular velocity, while using a smartphone in this study is, at least indirectly, in line with these previous findings. Importantly, impaired turning coordination and en bloc turning have been associated with the fear of falling and higher risk of falls [8,56]. Therefore, the result that all participants, irrespective of disease and age, and even young healthy adults, turn en bloc while using a smartphone could have an important impact on future recommendations for smartphone use while walking.

We found that participants with Parkinson disease showed the lowest head peak angular velocity in all conditions. They also showed the longest turn duration and needed most steps for the turns. This was an expected result considering that bradykinesia and rigidity as well as reduced gait speed and increased cadence with shorter step length are typical features of Parkinson disease [58,59]. Interestingly, participants with Parkinson disease also showed the most marked reduction in peak angular velocity of all 3 investigated body segments when using a smartphone during turning, reflected by the highest peak angular velocity DTC. The latter observation is, in our view, surprising and difficult to explain by the mere presence of bradykinesia and rigidity. We assume that this marked reduction in peak angular velocity is more of a cognitive phenomenon. Individuals with Parkinson disease have been reported to be particularly prone to experience DT deficits, particularly during walking [27,60]. Compared to walking, turning is a more complex and demanding task, and the simultaneous “triple task” (ie, smartphone use, turning, and cognitive task) could represent a bottleneck in neural processing that could lead to dangerous turning behavior, especially in individuals with Parkinson disease. For people with Parkinson disease experiencing falls, it could therefore be useful to consider that turning while using a smartphone constitutes a particular fall risk, and then to work out whether the risk is influenced more by the motor component, the cognitive component or by a combination of both.

Participants with lower-back pain were the only group that did not show any significant difference in head peak angular velocity when using a smartphone, compared with ST. This was particularly interesting due to the following observation. Although participants with Parkinson disease and those with lower-back pain were comparable concerning demographic and clinical (including pain intensity) parameters, both groups were remarkably different when comparing head peak angular velocity DTC (AUC of 0.96 and 0.92 for SDT and CDT, respectively, see also Multimedia Appendix 1). Considering that lower-back pain and similar pain conditions are common in prodromal [61,62] and clinically evident Parkinson disease [63,64], we speculate that the experimental paradigm presented here could be useful in differentiating individuals with lower-back pain without and those with (prodromal) Parkinson disease and future specifically designed studies could help confirming this hypothesis.

In our participants with subacute stroke, despite the head started turning slightly before sternum and pelvis, there was no significant difference in turning onset latencies among the 3 segment pairs even in the turning-while-walking–only condition. We hypothesize that en bloc turning may not be deleterious under all circumstances but, for example, in acute or subacute medical situations, may even serve as a compensative strategy to overcome the newly occurring mobility deficit. Increased cocontraction and impedance control with the goal of reducing kinematic errors, stabilizing movement, and increasing performance is a common strategy used in the early phases of motor learning when new dynamics have to be acquired [65]. Differing results from previous studies investigating intersegmental turning in individuals with stroke may, at least indirectly, also argue in this direction considering that all previous studies [12-16] included individuals with chronic stroke. Therefore, our results could pave the way for new rehabilitation strategies targeting gait and turning in individuals with subacute stroke.

Limitations

This study faces some limitations. First, the sample size at least of some groups, the range of disease severity, and the generally relatively high level of physical and cognitive abilities of our participants may limit the generalizability of our results, and further studies including participants with lower functional scores, greater disease severity, and more severe cognitive impairment may be helpful to address this. Second, in this proof-of-concept study, we chose a 180° turn paradigm. However, we are aware that other, primarily smaller turns are also performed in everyday life, and future studies should investigate the influence of smartphone use on these turns. Third, on average, the turning algorithm used in this study underestimated the turning magnitudes by 10%-15%. This could likely be attributed to the general structure of the algorithm (see the Methods section) in combination with the ellipse-like, “nonabrupt” turning trajectory. As this is a systematic bias observed in all experimental conditions and in all body segments, we assume that it is an irrelevant aspect of our data presented here, but future studies may use additional or alternative turning algorithms for data analysis. Finally, we included only smartphone tasks in which subjects had to interact with the touchscreen and did not include tasks in which participants had to walk while talking on the phone. The latter is also a very common type of smartphone use, and should therefore also be investigated concerning influence on turning behavior in future studies.

Conclusions

Performing a secondary task on a smartphone leads to a more en bloc turning irrespective of age and neurologic condition. The segmental turning behavior of participants with Parkinson disease suggests that this disease could be most affected by smartphone use and these participants could be at high risk of falling when turning while using a smartphone. Considering the ubiquitous smartphone use in daily life, results of this study could stimulate future studies in this area, as well as, in clinical routine, the type of history taken from elderly and neurologically ill individuals who have increased risk of falling during ambulation.

Acknowledgments

The authors sincerely thank all people that were involved in the data collection, from study participants to students who aided and assisted with the assessments. For this publication, we acknowledge financial support by DFG within the funding program Open Access Publikationskosten.

Data Availability

Data of a subgroup of healthy participants included in our study are published digitally [66] and are made available using Figshare. The rest of the data presented in this study are available on request from the corresponding author.

Authors' Contributions

EW, CH, and WM designed the study. RB and KS helped with participant recruitment and data collection. EW collected data. EW and EB performed data analyses. EB and WM wrote the first draft of the manuscript. EW, RR, SH, RB, KS, FEP, and CH reviewed the manuscript draft. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Conflicts of Interest

None declared.

Multimedia Appendix 1

Supplementary figures and tables.

DOCX File , 4015 KB

  1. Glaister BC, Bernatz GC, Klute GK, Orendurff MS. Video task analysis of turning during activities of daily living. Gait Posture 2007;25(2):289-294. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  2. Patla AE, Adkin A, Ballard T. Online steering: coordination and control of body center of mass, head and body reorientation. Exp Brain Res 1999;129(4):629-634. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  3. Crenna P, Carpinella I, Rabuffetti M, Calabrese E, Mazzoleni P, Nemni R, et al. The association between impaired turning and normal straight walking in Parkinson's disease. Gait Posture 2007;26(2):172-178. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  4. Fuller JR, Adkin AL, Vallis LA. Strategies used by older adults to change travel direction. Gait Posture 2007;25(3):393-400. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  5. Akram SB, Frank JS, Fraser J. Effect of walking velocity on segment coordination during pre-planned turns in healthy older adults. Gait Posture 2010;32(2):211-214. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  6. Robinovitch SN, Feldman F, Yang Y, Schonnop R, Leung PM, Sarraf T, et al. Video capture of the circumstances of falls in elderly people residing in long-term care: an observational study. Lancet 2013;381(9860):47-54 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  7. Pati D, Valipoor S, Cloutier A, Yang J, Freier P, Harvey TE, et al. Physical design factors contributing to patient falls. J Patient Saf 2021;17(3):e135-e142. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  8. Wright RL, Peters DM, Robinson PD, Sitch AJ, Watt TN, Hollands MA. Differences in axial segment reorientation during standing turns predict multiple falls in older adults. Gait Posture 2012;36(3):541-545. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  9. Khobkhun F, Hollands M, Richards J. The effect of different turn speeds on whole-body coordination in younger and older healthy adults. Sensors (Basel) 2021;21(8):33923838 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  10. Forsell C, Conradsson D, Paquette C, Franzén E. Reducing gait speed affects axial coordination of walking turns. Gait Posture 2017;54:71-75. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  11. Akram SB, Frank JS, Fraser J. Coordination of segments reorientation during on-the-spot turns in healthy older adults in eyes-open and eyes-closed conditions. Gait Posture 2010;32(4):632-636. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  12. Kim K, Fricke M, Bock O. Eye-head-trunk coordination while walking and turning in a simulated grocery shopping task. J Mot Behav 2021;53(5):575-582. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  13. Hollands KL, Hollands MA, Zietz D, Wing AM, Wright C, van Vliet P. Kinematics of turning 180 degrees during the timed up and go in stroke survivors with and without falls history. Neurorehabil Neural Repair 2010;24(4):358-367. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  14. Nakamura T, Higuchi T, Kikumoto T, Takeda T, Tashiro H, Hoshi F. Slower reorientation of trunk for reactive turning while walking in hemiparesis stroke patients. J Mot Behav 2019;51(6):640-646. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  15. Hollands KL, van Vliet P, Zietz D, Wing A, Wright C, Hollands MA. Stroke-related differences in axial body segment coordination during preplanned and reactive changes in walking direction. Exp Brain Res 2010;202(3):591-604. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  16. Ahmad RY, Ashburn A, Burnett M, Samuel D, Verheyden G. Sequence of onset latency of body segments when turning on-the-spot in people with stroke. Gait Posture 2014;39(3):841-846. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  17. Akram S, Frank JS, Jog M. Parkinson's disease and segmental coordination during turning: II. Walking turns. Can J Neurol Sci 2013;40(4):520-526. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  18. Hong M, Earhart GM. Effects of medication on turning deficits in individuals with Parkinson's disease. J Neurol Phys Ther 2010;34(1):11-16 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  19. Hong M, Perlmutter JS, Earhart GM. A kinematic and electromyographic analysis of turning in people with Parkinson disease. Neurorehabil Neural Repair 2009;23(2):166-176 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  20. Carpinella I, Crenna P, Calabrese E, Rabuffetti M, Mazzoleni P, Nemni R, et al. Locomotor function in the early stage of Parkinson's disease. IEEE Trans Neural Syst Rehabil Eng 2007;15(4):543-551. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  21. Ashburn A, Kampshoff C, Burnett M, Stack E, Pickering RM, Verheyden G. Sequence and onset of whole-body coordination when turning in response to a visual trigger: comparing people with Parkinson's disease and healthy adults. Gait Posture 2014;39(1):278-283. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  22. Lamontagne A, Fung J. Gaze and postural reorientation in the control of locomotor steering after stroke. Neurorehabil Neural Repair 2009;23(3):256-266. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  23. Manaf H, Justine M, Goh HT. Axial segmental coordination during turning: effects of stroke and attentional loadings. Motor Control 2017;21(1):42-57. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  24. Smith JA, Kulig K. Trunk-pelvis coordination during turning: a cross sectional study of young adults with and without a history of low back pain. Clin Biomech (Bristol, Avon) 2016;36:58-64 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  25. Smith JA, Gordon J, Kulig K. The influence of divided attention on walking turns: effects on gait control in young adults with and without a history of low back pain. Gait Posture 2017;58:498-503 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  26. Smith E, Cusack T, Cunningham C, Blake C. The influence of a cognitive dual task on the gait parameters of healthy older adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J Aging Phys Act 2017;25(4):671-686. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  27. Heinzel S, Maechtel M, Hasmann SE, Hobert MA, Heger T, Berg D, et al. Motor dual-tasking deficits predict falls in Parkinson's disease: a prospective study. Parkinsonism Relat Disord 2016;26:73-77. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  28. Leone C, Feys P, Moumdjian L, D'Amico E, Zappia M, Patti F. Cognitive-motor dual-task interference: a systematic review of neural correlates. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2017;75:348-360. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  29. Porciuncula FS, Rao AK, McIsaac TL. Aging-related decrements during specific phases of the dual-task Timed Up-and-Go test. Aging Clin Exp Res 2016;28(1):121-130 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  30. Hollands KL, Agnihotri D, Tyson SF. Effects of dual task on turning ability in stroke survivors and older adults. Gait Posture 2014;40(4):564-569 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  31. Bertoli M, Croce UD, Cereatti A, Mancini M. Objective measures to investigate turning impairments and freezing of gait in people with Parkinson's disease. Gait Posture 2019;74:187-193. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  32. Criminger C, Swank C. Impact of dual-tasking on mobility tasks in Parkinson's disease as described through 2D kinematic analysis. Aging Clin Exp Res 2020;32(5):835-840. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  33. Chan WN, Tsang WW. The performance of stroke survivors in turning-while-walking while carrying out a concurrent cognitive task compared with controls. PLoS One 2017;12(12):e0189800 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  34. Bruyneel AV, Duclos NC. Effects of the use of mobile phone on postural and locomotor tasks: a scoping review. Gait Posture 2020;82:233-241. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  35. Niederer D, Bumann A, Mühlhauser Y, Schmitt M, Wess K, Engeroff T, et al. Specific smartphone usage and cognitive performance affect gait characteristics during free-living and treadmill walking. Gait Posture 2018;62:415-421. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  36. Rubio Barañano A, Faisal M, Barrett BT, Buckley JG. Using a smartphone on the move: do visual constraints explain why we slow walking speed? Exp Brain Res 2022;240(2):467-480 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  37. Orhan E, Altın B, Aksoy S. Effect of smartphone use on static and dynamic postural balance in healthy young adults. Am J Audiol 2021;30(3):703-708. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  38. Gibb WR, Lees AJ. The relevance of the Lewy body to the pathogenesis of idiopathic Parkinson's disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1988;51(6):745-752 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  39. Thompson AJ, Banwell BL, Barkhof F, Carroll WM, Coetzee T, Comi G, et al. Diagnosis of multiple sclerosis: 2017 revisions of the McDonald criteria. Lancet Neurol 2018;17(2):162-173. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  40. Otto JC, Gierthmühlen J, Kirchhofer V, Borzikowsky C, Baron R. Validation of the questionnaire for symptom assessment in pain disorders for back pain patients (Q-SAP). Eur J Pain 2021;25(3):513-528. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  41. Nasreddine ZS, Phillips NA, Bédirian V, Charbonneau S, Whitehead V, Collin I, et al. The Montreal Cognitive Assessment, MoCA: a brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment. J Am Geriatr Soc 2005;53(4):695-699. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  42. Guralnik JM, Simonsick EM, Ferrucci L, Glynn RJ, Berkman LF, Blazer DG, et al. A short physical performance battery assessing lower extremity function: association with self-reported disability and prediction of mortality and nursing home admission. J Gerontol 1994;49(2):M85-M94. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  43. Goetz CG, Tilley BC, Shaftman SR, Stebbins GT, Fahn S, Martinez-Martin P, Movement Disorder Society UPDRS Revision Task Force. Movement Disorder Society-sponsored revision of the Unified Parkinson's Disease Rating Scale (MDS-UPDRS): scale presentation and clinimetric testing results. Mov Disord 2008;23(15):2129-2170. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  44. Hoehn MM, Yahr MD. Parkinsonism: onset, progression and mortality. Neurology 1967;17(5):427-442. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  45. Kurtzke JF. Rating neurologic impairment in multiple sclerosis: an expanded disability status scale (EDSS). Neurology 1983;33(11):1444-1452. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  46. Brott T, Adams HP, Olinger CP, Marler JR, Barsan WG, Biller J, et al. Measurements of acute cerebral infarction: a clinical examination scale. Stroke 1989;20(7):864-870. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  47. Williamson A, Hoggart B. Pain: a review of three commonly used pain rating scales. J Clin Nurs 2005;14(7):798-804. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  48. Haase I, Schwarz A, Burger A, Kladny B. Comparison of Hannover Functional Ability Questionnaire (FFbH) and the SF-36 subscale "physical functioning". Rehabilitation (Stuttg) 2001;40(1):40-42. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  49. Henik A, Tzelgov J. Is three greater than five: the relation between physical and semantic size in comparison tasks. Mem Cognit 1982;10(4):389-395. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  50. Warmerdam E, Romijnders R, Geritz J, Elshehabi M, Maetzler C, Otto JC, et al. Proposed mobility assessments with simultaneous full-body inertial measurement units and optical motion capture in healthy adults and neurological patients for future validation studies: study protocol. Sensors (Basel) 2021;21(17):5833 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  51. Wu G, van der Helm FC, Veeger HE, Makhsous M, Van Roy P, Anglin C, International Society of Biomechanics. ISB recommendation on definitions of joint coordinate systems of various joints for the reporting of human joint motion--part II: shoulder, elbow, wrist and hand. J Biomech 2005;38(5):981-992. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  52. O'Connor CM, Thorpe SK, O'Malley MJ, Vaughan CL. Automatic detection of gait events using kinematic data. Gait Posture 2007;25(3):469-474. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  53. Pijnappels M, Bobbert MF, van Dieën JH. Changes in walking pattern caused by the possibility of a tripping reaction. Gait Posture 2001;14(1):11-18. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  54. Bock O. Dual-task costs while walking increase in old age for some, but not for other tasks: an experimental study of healthy young and elderly persons. J Neuroeng Rehabil 2008;5:27 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  55. Kelly VE, Janke AA, Shumway-Cook A. Effects of instructed focus and task difficulty on concurrent walking and cognitive task performance in healthy young adults. Exp Brain Res 2010;207(1-2):65-73 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  56. Cocks AJ, Young WR, Ellmers TJ, Jackson RC, Williams AM. Concern about falling is associated with segmental control when turning in older adults. Gait Posture 2021;88:105-108. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  57. Kim SH, Jung JH, Shin HJ, Hahm SC, Cho HY. The impact of smartphone use on gait in young adults: cognitive load vs posture of texting. PLoS One 2020;15(10):e0240118 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  58. Armstrong MJ, Okun MS. Diagnosis and treatment of Parkinson disease: a review. JAMA 2020;323(6):548-560. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  59. Mirelman A, Bonato P, Camicioli R, Ellis TD, Giladi N, Hamilton JL, et al. Gait impairments in Parkinson's disease. Lancet Neurol 2019;18(7):697-708. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  60. Raffegeau TE, Krehbiel LM, Kang N, Thijs FJ, Altmann LJP, Cauraugh JH, et al. A meta-analysis: Parkinson's disease and dual-task walking. Parkinsonism Relat Disord 2019;62:28-35 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  61. Gaenslen A, Wurster I, Brockmann K, Huber H, Godau J, Faust B, et al. Prodromal features for Parkinson's disease--baseline data from the TREND study. Eur J Neurol 2014;21(5):766-772. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  62. Schrag A, Horsfall L, Walters K, Noyce A, Petersen I. Prediagnostic presentations of Parkinson's disease in primary care: a case-control study. Lancet Neurol 2015;14(1):57-64 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  63. Broetz D, Eichner M, Gasser T, Weller M, Steinbach JP. Radicular and nonradicular back pain in Parkinson's disease: a controlled study. Mov Disord 2007;22(6):853-856. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  64. Etchepare F, Rozenberg S, Mirault T, Bonnet AM, Lecorre C, Agid Y, et al. Back problems in Parkinson's disease: an underestimated problem. Joint Bone Spine 2006;73(3):298-302. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  65. Heald JB, Franklin DW, Wolpert DM. Increasing muscle co-contraction speeds up internal model acquisition during dynamic motor learning. Sci Rep 2018;8(1):16355 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  66. Warmerdam E, Hansen C, Romijnders R, Hobert MA, Welzel J, Maetzler W. Full-body mobility data to validate inertial measurement unit algorithms in healthy and neurological cohorts. Data 2022;7(10):136 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef]


CDT: complex dual task
DT: dual task
DTC: dual-task cost
MoCA: Montreal Cognitive Assessment
SDT: simple dual task
ST: single task


Edited by T Leung; submitted 14.07.22; peer-reviewed by H van Werkhoven, S Shanmukha, V Shah; comments to author 19.12.22; revised version received 09.01.23; accepted 29.01.23; published 30.03.23

Copyright

©Edoardo Bianchini, Elke Warmerdam, Robbin Romijnders, Klarissa Hanja Stürner, Ralf Baron, Sebastian Heinzel, Francesco Ernesto Pontieri, Clint Hansen, Walter Maetzler. Originally published in the Journal of Medical Internet Research (https://www.jmir.org), 30.03.2023.

This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work, first published in the Journal of Medical Internet Research, is properly cited. The complete bibliographic information, a link to the original publication on https://www.jmir.org/, as well as this copyright and license information must be included.