Published on in Vol 22, No 12 (2020): December

Preprints (earlier versions) of this paper are available at https://preprints.jmir.org/preprint/21923, first published .
Smartphone Overuse and Visual Impairment in Children and Young Adults: Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis

Smartphone Overuse and Visual Impairment in Children and Young Adults: Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis

Smartphone Overuse and Visual Impairment in Children and Young Adults: Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis

Authors of this article:

Jian Wang1 Author Orcid Image ;   Mei Li2 Author Orcid Image ;   Daqiao Zhu1 Author Orcid Image ;   Yang Cao3 Author Orcid Image

Review

1School of Nursing, Shanghai Jiao Tong University, Shanghai, China

2School of Medical Sciences, Örebro University, Örebro, Sweden

3Clinical Epidemiology and Biostatistics, School of Medical Sciences, Örebro University, Örebro, Sweden

Corresponding Author:

Yang Cao, PhD

Clinical Epidemiology and Biostatistics

School of Medical Sciences

Örebro University

Södra Grev Rosengatan 1

Örebro, 70182

Sweden

Phone: 46 196026236

Email: yang.cao@oru.se


Background: Smartphone overuse has been cited as a potentially modifiable risk factor that can result in visual impairment. However, reported associations between smartphone overuse and visual impairment have been inconsistent.

Objective: The aim of this systematic review was to determine the association between smartphone overuse and visual impairment, including myopia, blurred vision, and poor vision, in children and young adults.

Methods: We conducted a systematic search in the Cochrane Library, PubMed, EMBASE, Web of Science Core Collection, and ScienceDirect databases since the beginning of the databases up to June 2020. Fourteen eligible studies (10 cross-sectional studies and 4 controlled trials) were identified, which included a total of 27,110 subjects with a mean age ranging from 9.5 to 26.0 years. We used a random-effects model for meta-analysis of the 10 cross-sectional studies (26,962 subjects) and a fixed-effects model for meta-analysis of the 4 controlled trials (148 subjects) to combine odds ratios (ORs) and effect sizes (ES). The I2 statistic was used to assess heterogeneity.

Results: A pooled OR of 1.05 (95% CI 0.98-1.13, P=.16) was obtained from the cross-sectional studies, suggesting that smartphone overuse is not significantly associated with myopia, poor vision, or blurred vision; however, these visual impairments together were more apparent in children (OR 1.06, 95% CI 0.99-1.14, P=.09) than in young adults (OR 0.91, 95% CI 0.57-1.46,P=.71). For the 4 controlled trials, the smartphone overuse groups showed worse visual function scores compared with the reduced-use groups. The pooled ES was 0.76 (95% CI 0.53-0.99), which was statistically significant (P<.001).

Conclusions: Longer smartphone use may increase the likelihood of ocular symptoms, including myopia, asthenopia, and ocular surface disease, especially in children. Thus, regulating use time and restricting the prolonged use of smartphones may prevent ocular and visual symptoms. Further research on the patterns of use, with longer follow up on the longitudinal associations, will help to inform detailed guidelines and recommendations for smartphone use in children and young adults.

J Med Internet Res 2020;22(12):e21923

doi:10.2196/21923

Keywords



The use of smartphones has been increasing rapidly since their introduction in the late 2000s [1]. In 2019, the global smartphone penetration had reached approximately 41.5% of the global population [2]. Notably, the number of smartphone users in China was around 700 million in 2018, accounting for half of the Chinese population [3]. In addition, more than 80% of people in the United Kingdom owned or had ready access to a smartphone in 2019, representing a significant increase from 50% in 2012 [4]. Furthermore, more than 90% of young people between 16 and 34 years old in the United Kingdom owned a smartphone in 2019 [4].

With the continuous rise in youth digital media consumption, the incidence of ocular problems has also dramatically increased. A large portion of the population currently suffers from visual impairment, especially in Asian countries, with a rapidly increasing prevalence and younger age of onset [5-8]. It has been estimated that 49.8% (4.8 billion) and 9.8% (0.9 billion) of the global population will have myopia or high myopia by 2050 [9]. A recent study indicated that about 60 years ago, only 10%-20% of the Chinese population was nearsighted, but the percentage reached up to 90% of teenagers and young adults in 2015 [10]. Consistently, a school-based retrospective longitudinal cohort study (N=37,424 participants) found that the prevalence of myopia significantly increased from 56% in 2005 to 65% in 2015 [8].

Therefore, smartphone overuse among children and young adults has become a matter of crucial concern [11-13]. Several studies found increased use of digital devices in children aged 2-11 years old [14,15]. For example, a study including children aged 9-11 years from 12 countries showed that 54.2% of the children exceeded proposed screen time guidelines (≤2 hours per day) [15]. Compared with older people, children and young adults have greater risks of the undesirable consequences of smartphone overuse because they have less self-control in smartphone use [11]. A cross-sectional study (N=2639 participants) indicated that 22.8% of teenagers were addicted to smartphone use, which was related to hypertension [16]. Another study showed that users of mobile devices spent >20 hours weekly on email, text messages, and social networking services, indicating the heavy reliance on smartphones in their communication with other people [17]. Overall, smartphone overuse may result in significant harmful physical, psychological, and social consequences [18,19].

Some experimental studies have indicated that long-term use of a smartphone plays a key role in visual impairment, increasing the likelihood of poor vision [20-22]. For instance, a prospective clinical study (N=50 participants) showed that smartphone use for 4 hours resulted in a higher ocular surface disease index than that measured at baseline [20]. Kim et al [23] found that the increase of ocular symptoms extended to the general population, especially in adolescents, after expansion of smartphone use. However, other studies have reported the lack of evidence for such an association [24]. For example, a cross-sectional study (N=1153 participants) using stratified random cluster samples did not find a statistically significant association between smartphone use time and myopia [25]. Similarly, a study conducted in Ireland (N=418 participants) indicated that smartphone use time was not a risk factor for myopia [26]. Toh et al [27] found that smartphone use time was associated with an increased risk of visual symptoms (ie, blurring of vision, dry eye), but a decreased odds of myopia.

Despite increased concern about impaired vision due to smartphone overuse, existing quantitative evidence about the relationships between excessive smartphone use and visual impairment remains equivocal. Therefore, it is essential to confirm and quantify whether excessive smartphone use may result in visual impairment, especially in children and young adults.

The aim of this study was to conduct a systematic review and meta-analysis to summarize the existing evidence on the associations between smartphone overuse and visual impairment in children and young adults, which may further guide potential interventions to reduce the harmful impact of smartphone overuse on vision in this susceptible subpopulation.


Data Sources and Search Strategy

This systematic review and meta-analysis was based on a protocol designed in line with the standard Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analysis (PRISMA) [28] and Meta-analysis of Observational Studies in Epidemiology (MOOSE) [29] criteria.

A systematic search was carried out in PubMed (US National Library of Medicine), Embase (Wolters Kluwer Ovid), Web of Science Core Collection (Clarivate Analytics), ScienceDirect (Elsevier), and Cochrane library (John Wiley & Sons, Ltd) for observational and experimental studies that investigated smartphone overuse or addiction in children (aged<18 years) or young people (aged<40 years), and its associations with impaired visual function such as myopia, poor vision, or blurred vision. To minimize publication bias, we also searched for additional studies in grey literature sources, including Virtual Health Library [30], NARCIS [31], Grey literature report [32], and Open grey EU [33]. The search was limited to publications published in English.

Free text and Medical Subject Headings (MeSH) terms were used for the search, including phone, smartphone, mobile/cell/cellular phone, electronic device, use, use time, screen time, overuse, addiction, eye, visual acuity, vision, vision screening, eyesight, myopia, myopic refraction, shortsighted/nearsighted/short sight, near sight, refraction errors, ocular/health effect, optic, blind, ophthalmology, optometry, retina, ametropia/amblyopia symptom, visual assessment, and visual problem (see Multimedia Appendix 1 for the complete search strategy). We included all observational studies and controlled trials (randomized or nonrandomized) addressing smartphone use and visual impairment in humans since the beginning of the databases up to June 2020. Furthermore, manual retrieval was performed following the initial database search to ensure the inclusion of the latest literature.

Inclusion and Exclusion Criteria

All observational and experimental studies were included if they fulfilled the following criteria: (1) original studies examining the use of a smartphone (or mobile phone) and eyesight, including population-based longitudinal studies, cohort studies, case-control studies, cross-sectional studies, and controlled clinical trials; (2) participants are children aged ≤18 years or young people aged ≤40 years (a young adult was defined as the developmental stage between 18 and 40 years [34,35]); (3) reported frequency or time of smartphone use (in minutes or hours, or per day or week); (4) the endpoint of interest is the incidence of visual impairment or decline, including myopia, poor vision, blurred vision, various visual function scores indicating impaired vision, or other unspecific visual impairments; and (5) vision measurements of the groups are provided to calculate the effect size (ES) of visual impairment or odds ratio (OR) for the risk of visual impairment, as well as the associated 95% CIs or other data to estimate the variance or accuracy (eg, standard error).

Studies were excluded if they: (1) were narrative reviews, editorial papers, commentaries, letters, or methodological papers; (2) evaluated visual function with no reliable/relevant estimates for smartphone use; (3) no reference or control group was included in the analysis; and (4) animal studies.

Data Extraction

After the systematic search of the relevant articles in the databases, two investigators (JW and ML) embarked on screening and identification of potentially relevant abstracts independently. For any disagreements that occurred between the two investigators regarding the eligibility of a study, there was a thorough discussion or advice from an academic expert (YC). Subsequently, articles for selected abstracts were downloaded, and data were extracted by JW and YC independently using a standardized form in Microsoft Excel. The extracted data were compared and summarized to obtain one final document from which the analysis was conducted. The information extracted included: name of first author, year of publication, study design, duration of study, country that the study was conducted in, eyesight measurement, smartphone use time, smartphone use frequency, sample size, incidence of cases with impaired vision, outcome ascertainment method, OR or ES and the associated 95% CI, and statistical analysis method used.

Study Quality Assessment

The Joanna Briggs Institute (JBI) Critical Appraisal Checklist for Analytical Cross Sectional Studies, JBI Appraisal Checklist for Quasi-Experimental Studies, and JBI Critical Appraisal Checklist for Randomized Controlled Trials were used to assess the quality of the studies included in the meta-analysis [36]. JW and YC assessed the quality of the articles independently and the final assessment was achieved upon discussion (Multimedia Appendix 2).

Statistical Analysis

For studies that did not report the OR, it was calculated using the numbers of cases with and without visual impairment of the reference/control group and overuse group. For studies that measured visual impairment using continuous variables, ES was calculated as the difference between the means divided by the pooled SD as follows [37]:

where n1 and n2, and S1 and S2 are the sample sizes and standard deviations for group 1 and group 2, respectively.

A positive ES indicates a worse visual function. Heterogeneity of the included studies was investigated using the I2 statistic [38], in which I2>30% was considered to indicate moderate heterogeneity and I2>50% was considered to indicate substantial heterogeneity [39]. A P value <.05 from the noncentral chi-squared test for heterogeneity was considered to indicate statistically significant heterogeneity [40]. The contribution of each study to the heterogeneity and their influence on the pooled OR or ES were assessed using the Baujat plot [41]. The pooled ORs with corresponding 95% CIs were calculated using random-effects models owing to heterogeneity among the studies and are presented using forest plots [42]. The possibility of publication bias was assessed by the combination of the Egger test and visual inspection of the funnel plot [43].

Subgroup analysis was performed for the cross-sectional studies according to the outcome of visual impairment (myopia, poor vision, or blurred vision) and mean age of the subjects (children, ≤18 years; young people, 18-40 years). Leave-one-out (LOO) analysis was also performed to investigate the influence of a single study on the pooled effect as an additional sensitivity analysis [44].

A two-sided P value <.05 of the pooled estimates was considered statistically significant unless otherwise specified. All analyses were performed in R 4.0.0 (R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria) using the packages meta 4.12-0 [45] and dmetar 0.0.9000 [46].


Characteristics of Included Studies

In total, 1961 articles were obtained from all of the databases. After removing duplicates, 1796 articles remained, 121 of which were considered to be relevant for the meta-analysis after screening of titles and abstracts. After screening the full text of the 121 articles downloaded, 14 articles met our inclusion criteria, including 10 cross-sectional studies and 4 controlled trials, comprising a total of 27,110 participants with mean ages ranging from 9.5 to 26.0 years. The flowchart of article searching and screening is shown in Figure 1. The 10 cross-sectional studies addressed incidents of myopia [24-27,47], blurred vision [48-50], and poor vision and other unspecified visual impairments [23,27,48,51]. In our analysis, the unspecified visual impairments were treated as poor vision. There were 2 studies [27,48] that addressed two visual impairment outcomes, and each outcome was treated as a single study in the meta-analysis. The 4 studies that used a controlled trial design assessed the ocular surface disease index score [20], asthenopia score [21], oculomotor function [52], and viewing distance [22]. A more detailed summary of the characteristics of the included studies and the main outcomes is provided in Table 1 and Table 2, respectively.

Figure 1. PRISMA (Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses) flow diagram for screening and selection of articles on smartphone overuse and visual impairment in children and young adults.
View this figure
Table 1. General characteristics of the included studies.
ReferenceYearCountryStudy designAge of participants (years), mean (SD) or rangeSampling methodN participants
Küçer et al [49]2008TurkeyCross-sectionalUniversity students (age not given)Convenience sample229
Toh et al [27]2019SingaporeCross-sectional13.3 (2.0)Matrix-stratified sample1884
Merrie et al [51]2019EthiopiaCross-sectional13.1 (2.8)Multistage sampling601
Guan et al [47]2019ChinaCross-sectional10.6 (1.15)Randomly selected sample19,934
Kim et al [23]2016KoreaCross-sectional15 (0.9)Convenience sample715
Liu et al [24]2019ChinaCross-sectional9.5 (2.1)Stratified
cluster sample
566
Meo et al [50]2005Saudi ArabiaCross-sectional26.0 (13.4)Voluntary (response) sample873
Alharbi et al [48]2019Saudi ArabiaCross-sectional21.8 (2.4)Random sample605
Huang et al [25]2019ChinaCross-sectional19.6 (0.9)Stratified random cluster sample1153
McCrann et al [26]2020IrelandCross-sectional16.8 (4.4)Voluntary sample402
Antona et al [21]2018SpainRCTa23.7 (2.6)Random sample54
Choi et al [20]2018South KoreaCTb26.0 (3.0)Nonrandomized sample50
Lee et al [52]2019KoreaCT20-29Voluntary sample26
Long et al [22]2017AustraliaCT21.5 (3.3)Voluntary sample18

aRCT: randomized controlled trial.

bCT: controlled trial.

Table 2. Outcomes and results of the included studies.
ReferenceResponse rateExposure; type of measureOutcome; type of measureMain results
Küçer et al [49]100%Time of mobile phone possession; QaBlurred vision; Q≤2 years: 8.8% (4/45)
>2 years: 27.2% (50/184)
Toh et al [27]93.78% (1884/2009)Time of smartphone use (per hour); Q(1) Myopia; Q
(2) Poor vision/visual impairment; Q
(1) ORb 0.97 (95% CI 0.94-0.99)
(2) OR 1.05 (95% CI 1.02-1.08)
Merrie et al [51]95.09% (601/632)Duration of mobile exposure; QPoor vision/visual impairment; objective assessment>2 h/day: 6.6% (18/271)
≤ 2 h/day: 7.5% (20/265)
Guan et al [47]UKcTime of smartphone use; QVisual acuity; objective assessment1 h/day: 20% (117/584);
≤1 h/day: 18% (3492/19350)
Kim et al [23]97.41% (715/734)Time of smartphone use; QPoor vision/ocular symptom score; Q

>2 h/day: 72% (260/360);
≤2 h/day: 52% (170/327)
Liu et al [24]88.7% (566/638)Time of smartphone use (per hour); QMyopia; objective assessmentOR 0.90 (95% CI 0.57-1.43)
Meo et al [50]100%Use of mobile phone (duration of calls); QBlurred vision; Q>0.5 h/day: 5% (5/100);
≤0.5 h/day:
5.23% (39/746)
Alharbi et al [48]93.1% (605/650)Duration of Smartphone use per day; Q(1) Poor vision; Q
(2) Blurred vision; Q

(1) >3 h/day: 57.2% (270/472);
≤3 h/day: 45.9% (61/133)
(2) >3 h/day: 46.0% (217/472);
≤3 h/day: 57.1% (76/133)
Huang et al [25]96.08% (1153/1200)Duration of daily smartphone use; QMyopia; objective assessment>3 h/day: 84.57% (296/350); ≤ 3 h/day: 88.03% (537/610)
McCrann et al [26]96.17% (402/418)Time on phone (minutes/day); QMyopia; QOR 1.026 (95% CI 1.001-1.051)
Antona et al [21]100%Smartphone reading vs printed hardcopy readingAsthenopia score; Q27.96 (SD 20.11) vs 13.25 (SD 12.76)
Choi et al [20]100%Smartphone use after 4 hours vs baselineOcular surface disease index scores; Q25.03 (SD 10.61) vs 15.08 (SD 8.83)
Lee et al [52]86.67% (26/30)Smartphone use 20 minutes vs 5 minutesOculomotor function; Q6.35 (SD 3.54) vs 3.73 (SD 4.09)
Long et al [22]100%Using smartphone after 1 hour vs baselineViewing distance; objective assessment27.8 (SD 7.7) cm vs 31 (SD 8.2) cm

aQ: questionnaire.

bOR: odds ratio.

cUK: unknown.

Association Between Smartphone Overuse and Incidence of Visual Impairment

The funnel plot of ORs for the included cross-sectional studies appeared to be symmetric (Figure 2). Although ORs from two studies [23,49] showed slight bias with other studies, no statistically significant publication bias was found based on the Egger test (P=.43).

Figure 2. Funnel plot with pseudo 95% confidence limit for cross-sectional studies.
View this figure

Statistically significant heterogeneity was present among the ORs on visual impairment incidence (I2=84%, P<.001; Figure 3). The Baujat plot indicated that the study of Kim et al [23] substantially contributed to the heterogeneity but had a minimal influence on the pooled OR (Figure 4). Overall, although the pooled OR showed that the odds of visual impairment was higher for the smartphone overuse group compared to the reduced-use group (OR 1.05, 95% CI 0.98-1.13), the result was not statistically significant (P=.16; Figure 3). None of the pooled ORs for specific visual impairment was significant in subgroup analyses. The pooled ORs for myopia, poor vision, and blurred vision were 1.00 (95% CI 0.95-1.05), 1.40 (95% CI 0.87-2.23), and 1.21 (95% CI 0.44-3.28), respectively (Figure 3). The pooled OR was not statistically significant in either of the age subgroups, which was 1.06 (95% CI 0.99-1.14, P=.09) for children and 0.91 (95% CI 0.57-1.46, P=.71) for young adults.

Figure 3. Pooled odds ratios (ORs) of visual impairment in the smartphone overuse group compared to the reduced-use group.
View this figure
Figure 4. Baujat plot for cross-sectional studies.
View this figure

The LOO sensitivity test indicated that ORs of visual impairment in the smartphone overuse group compared to the reduced-use group ranged from 1.02 to 1.09; however, none of the ORs was statistically significant (Figure 5).

Figure 5. Pooled odds ratios (ORs) of visual impairment in the smartphone overuse group compared to the reduced-use group from leave-one-out analysis.
View this figure

Smartphone Overuse Associated With Worse Visual Function Scores

The funnel plot of ES for the included controlled trials appeared to be symmetric (Figure 6), and no statistically significant publication bias was found by the Egger test (P=.067). No statistically significant heterogeneity was present among the ESs on visual impairment incidence (I2=0%, P=.54; Figure 7).

Figure 6. Funnel plot with pseudo 95% confidence limit for controlled trials.
View this figure

In all of the controlled trials, the smartphone overuse group showed worse visual function scores than the reduced-use group, with ESs ranging from 0.40 to 0.91 (Figure 7). The pooled ES was 0.76 (95% CI 0.53-0.99), which was statistically significant (P<.001), indicating that compared with the reduced-use group, the visual function score in the smartphone overuse group was 0.76 SD worse (Figure 7).

Figure 7. Pooled effect size (ES) of visual function score in the smartphone overuse group compared to the reduced-use group.
View this figure

The LOO sensitivity test indicated that the results are robust, with the ESs ranging from 0.65 to 0.82, and all of the ESs were statistically significant (Figure 8).

Figure 8. Pooled effect sizes (ESs) of visual function score in the smartphone overuse group compared to the reduced-use group from leave-one-out analysis.
View this figure

Principal Findings

The purpose of this systematic review and meta-analysis was to summarize currently available evidence with reference to the relationship between smartphone overuse and visual impairment in children and young adults. Among the 14 studies included in the analysis, 9 found a significant association between smartphone overuse and visual impairment. Our pooled results showed negative but not statistically significant associations (OR=1.05, 95% CI 0.98-1.13) between smartphone overuse and myopia, blurred vision, or poor vision in the included cross-sectional studies. However, the adverse effect was more apparent in children (OR=1.06, 95% CI 0.99-1.14) than in young adults (OR=0.91, 95% CI 0.57-1.46). We also found that smartphone overuse may cause worse visual function than reduced use in the included controlled trials (ES=0.76, 95% CI 0.53-0.99). As the results are mixed, further studies are warranted. To our knowledge, this is the first systematic review that comprehensively summarized existing data on smartphone overuse and visual impairment in children and young adults.

There are several possible reasons for the lack of a statistically significant association observed between smartphone overuse and visual impairment when pooling cross-sectional studies. First, most of the existing studies included in this systematic review were from Asia, which has higher prevalence rates of visual impairment. The myopia prevalence in East Asia was already reported to be high before the introduction of digital devices [53]. Previous studies indicated that myopia prevalence increased more rapidly in people with more years of education and intensive schooling without particular exposure to screen devices [54-56]. For example, a study conducted in Singapore found that myopia prevalence increased more rapidly among individuals who started elementary school after the 1980s [57]. Consistently, a study in Israel found that teenage boys who attended Orthodox schools had much higher rates of myopia than students from other schools who spent less time reading books in the 1990s [53]. Therefore, education and intensive schooling may have a large contribution to the increase in myopia prevalence [58]. Recent studies have also extensively described the relationship between education and visual impairment [59]. Furthermore, a high prevalence of myopia from Taiwan was found in cohorts with low exposure to digital devices [56]. Thus, it is still debatable whether smartphone overuse would lead to a higher risk of myopia or other visual problems.

Second, most of the studies included in this analysis divided smartphone overuse as use time over 2 or 3 hours per day. However, there is some evidence that the time people actually spend engaged with a digital screen is far longer [60-62], suggesting that people may use other electronic devices. Overuse of other digital devices might also play an important role in visual impairment. Some studies have explored the relationships between digital screen time (eg, computer, tablet, smartphone, or other handheld electronic screens) and visual impairment [58,61,63-66]. For instance, a birth cohort study (N=5074 participants) showed that increased computer use was associated with myopia development in children [65]. Yang et al [63] found that screen exposure was significantly and positively associated with preschool myopia, which is consistent with the results of another cohort study [66]. However, the results of studies assessing the impacts of screen time on visual impairment have been mixed. A recent systematic review showed that screen time was not significantly associated with the prevalence and incidence of myopia [58], which may largely support our pooled result of cross-sectional studies. Thus, the relationship needs to be further validated. Moreover, given differences in the use of various digital devices, some studies have compared the impacts of smartphone use with other digital devices on visual impairment [20,24,25,27]. These results are also inconsistent. For instance, Guan et al [47] (N=19,934 participants) found that prolonged (>60 minutes/day) computer usage and smartphone usage were both significantly associated with greater refractive error. Nevertheless, Liu et al [24] and Huang et al [25] found that myopia in children was not associated with time spent using various electronic devices, including smartphones, tablets, and computers. By contrast, a study with a representative sample of 1884 adolescents showed that smartphone use time was associated with an increased risk of visual symptoms, but no significant association was found for tablet use [27]. A controlled trial (N=50 participants) indicated that the smartphone use group had higher fatigue, burning, and dryness scores than the computer use group [20]. Although the existing research supports that smartphone use might cause worse vision than other digital devices, further convincing evidence is needed to support this conclusion owing to the low number of studies.

Third, several studies have shown that technology use or screen time alone is of minimal risk to visual impairment, whereas more time spent outdoors is related to a reduced risk of myopia and myopic progression [25,67,68]. However, there is also evidence that the increased use of digital devices is associated with more time at work and less time spent outdoors, resulting in a substitution effect [58,69]. For example, Dirani et al [69] reported that the lack of adequate outdoor activity might be related to the rise in digital screen time. More specifically, recent educational screen time might be a replacement for reading or writing, in addition to recreational screen time (eg, computer or video games) [69]. For instance, smartphones are used by children mainly for playing games (29%) and watching videos (20%) but also for learning (19%) [70]. Thus, digital screen time might not be a causal factor, but may be a substitute for a different types of work [58]. There is also some evidence that children 9-11 years old who spent less than 2 hours playing on a computer were 1.98 times more likely to spend more than 1 hour outside than those reporting 2 or more hours of computer use [71]. Although these results might reflect a tradeoff between outdoor time and digital screen time, with screen time being a proxy for indoor time, there is no evidence to confirm this substitution effect [58]. Thus, further studies in this field are warranted.

Besides the findings in the cross-sectional studies, we also found that the smartphone overuse group presented worse visual function scores than the reduced-use group in each of the included controlled trials and in the pooled result. Biologically, the effects of smartphones on ocular symptoms can be explained by two types of electromagnetic fields (EMFs): extremely low-frequency EMFs and radiofrequency (RF) electromagnetic radiation (EMR) [72,73]. The intensity of radiation from mobile phones is relatively low with a specific absorption rate <4 W/kg [72,74]. However, it has been reported that adverse effects such as DNA damage and thickening of the cornea occur even at a specific absorption rate lower than 4 W/kg [72,75,76]. The local specific absorption rate has been shown to be higher in tissues at a younger age, suggesting higher susceptibility of adolescents to smartphones [77]. The EMFs generated by smartphones may interact with the tissues of the eyes [73,78], which may cause apoptosis, cataract formation, edema, endothelial cell loss, inflammatory responses, and neurological effects [72,74,79,80]. The RF EMR may affect the body thermally and nonthermally [81], which may result in oxidative stress in the cornea and the lens [74]. These effects by EMFs and RF EMR on the eyes, especially on the cornea and the lens, could suggest why ocular symptoms such as blurring, redness, visual disturbance, inflammation, and lacrimation increase with more exposure to smartphones [23]. Although experimental studies may provide causal inferences, our result needs to be further confirmed due to the limited number of existing studies.

Regarding the association between smartphone overuse and myopia examined in the cross-sectional studies, multiple ocular symptoms found in the experimental studies do not necessarily reflect pathological changes in the eyes, such as myopia. Few longitudinal cohort studies have examined the impacts of screen exposure on myopia, and the results are inconsistent [66,82]. To our knowledge, there have been no experimental or longitudinal studies detecting the impacts of smartphone overuse on myopia specifically. Thus, a longitudinal cohort study design establishing the temporal sequence of prior exposure to environmental factors would be useful to examine whether smartphone overuse may increase the risk of developing myopia.

In addition, the heterogeneity was high in the meta-analysis of included cross-sectional studies. First, a large number of studies have identified potential risk factors that may result in visual impairment, which included both genetic and environmental factors [20,26,56,58] such as age [26], education and occupation [58], outdoor activity [20,58], and parental myopia [20]. However, some studies did not include these variables in the multivariate analysis, which might contribute to the inconsistent findings, and might further affect the individual effect estimates and the pooled OR. Second, some studies only used univariate analysis to investigate the associations between smartphone use time and visual impairment [47,69], which might hinder the exploration of their interrelationships. Third, the assessment of the outcome was inconsistent. For example, some studies used a self-reported questionnaire to identify myopia [26,27], while others used an objective assessment [24,25]. Furthermore, the division of smartphone overuse was inconsistent, which may have precluded us from determining their significant relationships. A guideline advised limiting recreational screen time to no more than 2 hours per day [83]. Therefore, further studies in this field should use a broadly recognized standard to define smartphone overuse.

There are also other limitations of this study that need to be addressed. All of the included studies used a self-reported questionnaire to evaluate smartphone use time. Participants in the included experimental studies also mostly reported their visual function using questionnaires. The questionnaires themselves may be a potential source of error due to inaccurate reporting or recall bias of the participants. Further research should adopt objective instruments to measure smartphone use time and visual acuity screening to examine visual function. Furthermore, generalization of the results should be interpreted with caution owing to the low number of studies included in each meta-analysis. Limiting the review to studies reported in English may have also resulted in nonreporting of studies published in other languages. Nevertheless, our review involved rigorous methodological procedures to obtain and pool data from 27,110 children and young adults. We also adopted a wide range of search terms to retrieve all potential articles published in English, including the grey literature, which might have helped to reduce the publication bias in the final combination.

Conclusions

Overall, current evidence suggests that the results of the association between smartphone overuse and visual impairment in children and young adults are mixed. Although the statistically significantly negative association between smartphone overuse and visual impairment in the meta-analysis was only confirmed in controlled trials and not in cross-sectional studies, the adverse effect of smartphone overuse on visual functions was more apparent in children. However, these relationships need to be further verified. Further research on the patterns of use, with longer follow-up periods to detect longitudinal associations, and the exact mechanisms underlying these associations will help inform detailed guidelines for smartphone use in children and young adults. In addition, understanding the factors of smartphone overuse that account for the risk of ocular symptoms could help the growing population of smartphone users, especially children and young adults, to use smartphones in a healthier manner.

Authors' Contributions

Conceptualization: DZ, ML, and YC; Data curation: JW, ML, and YC; Formal analysis: JW and YC; Investigation: JW, DZ, and ML; Methodology: DZ and YC; Project administration: YC; Software: YC; Supervision: DZ and YC; Validation: JW and YC; Visualization: YC; Writing – original draft: JW and YC; Writing – review & editing: JW, DZ, ML, and YC.

Conflicts of Interest

None declared.

Multimedia Appendix 1

Literature search strategy and results.

DOCX File , 20 KB

Multimedia Appendix 2

Tables of study quality assessment.

DOC File , 69 KB

  1. Böhm S. An analysis on country-specific characteristics influencing smartphone usage. 2015 Presented at: First International Conference on Multidisciplinary in Management; 2015; Bangkok p. 335-348.
  2. Ertemel A, Ari E. A Marketing Approach to a Psychological Problem: Problematic Smartphone Use on Adolescents. Int J Environ Res Public Health 2020 Apr 04;17(7):2471 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  3. Zhai X, Ye M, Wang C, Gu Q, Huang T, Wang K, et al. Associations among physical activity and smartphone use with perceived stress and sleep quality of Chinese college students. Ment Health Phys Act 2020 Mar;18:100323. [CrossRef]
  4. Csibi S, Griffiths MD, Demetrovics Z, Szabo A. Analysis of Problematic Smartphone Use Across Different Age Groups within the ‘Components Model of Addiction’. Int J Ment Health Addiction 2019 May 23:1-16. [CrossRef]
  5. Li L, Zhong H, Li J, Li C, Pan C. Incidence of myopia and biometric characteristics of premyopic eyes among Chinese children and adolescents. BMC Ophthalmol 2018 Jul 20;18(1):178 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  6. Wang SK, Guo Y, Liao C, Chen Y, Su G, Zhang G, et al. Incidence of and Factors Associated With Myopia and High Myopia in Chinese Children, Based on Refraction Without Cycloplegia. JAMA Ophthalmol 2018 Sep 01;136(9):1017-1024 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  7. Grzybowski A, Kanclerz P, Tsubota K, Lanca C, Saw S. A review on the epidemiology of myopia in school children worldwide. BMC Ophthalmol 2020 Jan 14;20(1):27 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  8. Li Y, Liu J, Qi P. The increasing prevalence of myopia in junior high school students in the Haidian District of Beijing, China: a 10-year population-based survey. BMC Ophthalmol 2017 Jun 12;17(1):88 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  9. Holden BA, Fricke TR, Wilson DA, Jong M, Naidoo KS, Sankaridurg P, et al. Global Prevalence of Myopia and High Myopia and Temporal Trends from 2000 through 2050. Ophthalmology 2016 May;123(5):1036-1042 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  10. Dolgin E. The myopia boom. Nature 2015 Mar 19;519(7543):276-278. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  11. Choi S, Kim D, Choi J, Ahn H, Choi E, Song W, et al. Comparison of risk and protective factors associated with smartphone addiction and Internet addiction. J Behav Addict 2015 Dec;4(4):308-314 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  12. Liu C, Lin S, Pan Y, Lin Y. Smartphone gaming and frequent use pattern associated with smartphone addiction. Medicine (Baltimore) 2016 Jul;95(28):e4068 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  13. Haug S, Castro RP, Kwon M, Filler A, Kowatsch T, Schaub MP. Smartphone use and smartphone addiction among young people in Switzerland. J Behav Addict 2015 Dec;4(4):299-307 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  14. Bernard JY, Padmapriya N, Chen B, Cai S, Tan KH, Yap F, et al. Predictors of screen viewing time in young Singaporean children: the GUSTO cohort. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 2017 Sep 05;14(1):112 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  15. LeBlanc AG, Katzmarzyk PT, Barreira TV, Broyles ST, Chaput J, Church TS, ISCOLE Research Group. Correlates of Total Sedentary Time and Screen Time in 9-11 Year-Old Children around the World: The International Study of Childhood Obesity, Lifestyle and the Environment. PLoS One 2015;10(6):e0129622 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  16. Zou Y, Xia N, Zou Y, Chen Z, Wen Y. Smartphone addiction may be associated with adolescent hypertension: a cross-sectional study among junior school students in China. BMC Pediatr 2019 Sep 04;19(1):310 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  17. Lee S, Kang H, Shin G. Head flexion angle while using a smartphone. Ergonomics 2015 Oct 17;58(2):220-226. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  18. van Deursen AJ, Bolle CL, Hegner SM, Kommers PA. Modeling habitual and addictive smartphone behavior. Comput Hum Behav 2015 Apr;45:411-420. [CrossRef]
  19. Pan C, Qiu Q, Qian D, Hu D, Li J, Saw S, et al. Iris colour in relation to myopia among Chinese school-aged children. Ophthalmic Physiol Opt 2018 Jan 20;38(1):48-55. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  20. Choi JH, Li Y, Kim SH, Jin R, Kim YH, Choi W, et al. The influences of smartphone use on the status of the tear film and ocular surface. PLoS One 2018 Oct 31;13(10):e0206541 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  21. Antona B, Barrio AR, Gascó A, Pinar A, González-Pérez M, Puell MC. Symptoms associated with reading from a smartphone in conditions of light and dark. Appl Ergon 2018 Apr;68:12-17. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  22. Long J, Cheung R, Duong S, Paynter R, Asper L. Viewing distance and eyestrain symptoms with prolonged viewing of smartphones. Clin Exp Optom 2017 Mar 08;100(2):133-137. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  23. Kim J, Hwang Y, Kang S, Kim M, Kim TS, Kim J, et al. Association between Exposure to Smartphones and Ocular Health in Adolescents. Ophthalmic Epidemiol 2016 Aug;23(4):269-276. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  24. Liu S, Ye S, Xi W, Zhang X. Electronic devices and myopic refraction among children aged 6-14 years in urban areas of Tianjin, China. Ophthalmic Physiol Opt 2019 Jul 17;39(4):282-293. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  25. Huang L, Kawasaki H, Liu Y, Wang Z. The prevalence of myopia and the factors associated with it among university students in Nanjing: A cross-sectional study. Medicine (Baltimore) 2019 Mar;98(10):e14777. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  26. McCrann S, Loughman J, Butler JS, Paudel N, Flitcroft DI. Smartphone use as a possible risk factor for myopia. Clin Exp Optom 2020 May 25:online ahead of print. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  27. Toh SH, Coenen P, Howie EK, Mukherjee S, Mackey DA, Straker LM. Mobile touch screen device use and associations with musculoskeletal symptoms and visual health in a nationally representative sample of Singaporean adolescents. Ergonomics 2019 Jun 23;62(6):778-793. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  28. Moher D, Liberati A, Tetzlaff J, Altman DG, PRISMA Group. Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: the PRISMA statement. Int J Surg 2010;8(5):336-341 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  29. Stroup DF, Berlin JA, Morton SC, Olkin I, Williamson GD, Rennie D, et al. Meta-analysis of observational studies in epidemiology: a proposal for reporting. Meta-analysis Of Observational Studies in Epidemiology (MOOSE) group. JAMA 2000 Apr 19;283(15):2008-2012. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  30. VHL Regional Portal.   URL: https://bvsalud.org/en/ [accessed 2020-11-18]
  31. NARCIS. Data Archiving and Networked Services (DANS).   URL: https://www.narcis.nl [accessed 2020-11-18]
  32. Grey Literature Report. The New York Academy of Medicine.   URL: http://greylit.org [accessed 2020-11-18]
  33. Open Grey.   URL: http://opengrey.eu [accessed 2020-11-18]
  34. Erikson E, Erikson J. The life cycle completed (extended version). New York: WW Norton & Company; 1998.
  35. Zastrow C, Kirst-Ashman K. Understanding human behavior and the social environment. Hampshire: Cengage Learning; 2006.
  36. Critical Appraisal Tools. Joanna Briggs Institute. 2017.   URL: https://joannabriggs.org/critical-appraisal-tools [accessed 2020-11-28]
  37. Weed LL. Medical records that guide and teach. N Engl J Med 1968 Mar 21;278(12):652-657. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  38. Higgins JPT, Thompson SG, Deeks JJ, Altman DG. Measuring inconsistency in meta-analyses. BMJ 2003 Sep 06;327(7414):557-560 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  39. Higgins J, Green S. Cochrane handbook for systematic reviews of interventions Version 5.1. London: The Cochrane Collaboration; 2018.
  40. Higgins JPT, Thompson SG. Quantifying heterogeneity in a meta-analysis. Stat Med 2002 Jun 15;21(11):1539-1558. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  41. Baujat B, Mahé C, Pignon J, Hill C. A graphical method for exploring heterogeneity in meta-analyses: application to a meta-analysis of 65 trials. Stat Med 2002 Sep 30;21(18):2641-2652. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  42. DerSimonian R, Laird N. Meta-analysis in clinical trials. Control Clin Trials 1986 Sep;7(3):177-188. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  43. Egger M, Davey Smith G, Schneider M, Minder C. Bias in meta-analysis detected by a simple, graphical test. BMJ 1997 Sep 13;315(7109):629-634 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  44. Tobias A. Assessing the influence of a single study in the meta-analysis estimate. Stata Tech Bull 1999;8(47):15-17.
  45. Schwarzer G, Carpenter J, Rücker G. Meta-analysis with R. Berlin: Springer; 2015.
  46. Harrer M, Cuijpers P, Furukawa T, Ebert DD. dmetar: Companion R package for the Guide ‘Doing meta-analysis is R’.   URL: https://dmetar.protectlab.org/reference/dmetar.html [accessed 2020-11-28]
  47. Guan H, Yu NN, Wang H, Boswell M, Shi Y, Rozelle S, et al. Impact of various types of near work and time spent outdoors at different times of day on visual acuity and refractive error among Chinese school-going children. PLoS One 2019 Apr 26;14(4):e0215827 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  48. Alharbi M, Alem A, Alrizgi H, Abualnassr K, Abualnassr F, Alshencieti A. Impact of Smartphones on Eye Health among Health Sciences Students in Taibah University, Saudi Arabia. Indo Am J Pharm Sci 2019;6(2):3523-3530. [CrossRef]
  49. Küçer N. Some ocular symptoms experienced by users of mobile phones. Electromagn Biol Med 2008 Jul 07;27(2):205-209. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  50. Meo SA, Al-Drees AM. Mobile phone related-hazards and subjective hearing and vision symptoms in the Saudi population. Int J Occup Med Environ Health 2005;18(1):53-57. [Medline]
  51. Merrie YA, Tegegne MM, Munaw MB, Alemu HW. Prevalence And Associated Factors Of Visual Impairment Among School-Age Children In Bahir Dar City, Northwest Ethiopia. Clin Optom (Auckl) 2019;11:135-143. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  52. Lee D, Hong S, Jung S, Lee K, Lee G. The Effects of Viewing Smart Devices on Static Balance, Oculomotor Function, and Dizziness in Healthy Adults. Med Sci Monit 2019 Oct 27;25:8055-8060 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  53. Zylbermann R, Landau D, Berson D. The influence of study habits on myopia in Jewish teenagers. J Pediatr Ophthalmol Strabismus 1993;30(5):319-322. [Medline]
  54. Lin LLK, Shih YF, Hsiao CK, Chen CJ. Prevalence of myopia in Taiwanese schoolchildren: 1983 to 2000. Ann Acad Med Singap 2004 Jan;33(1):27-33 [FREE Full text] [Medline]
  55. Morgan I, Rose K. How genetic is school myopia? Prog Retin Eye Res 2005 Jan;24(1):1-38. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  56. Morgan IG, French AN, Ashby RS, Guo X, Ding X, He M, et al. The epidemics of myopia: Aetiology and prevention. Prog Retin Eye Res 2018 Jan;62:134-149. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  57. Sensaki S, Sabanayagam C, Verkicharla PK, Awodele A, Tan KH, Chia A, et al. An Ecologic Study of Trends in the Prevalence of Myopia in Chinese Adults in Singapore Born from the 1920s to 1980s. Ann Acad Med Singap 2017 Jun;46(6):229-236 [FREE Full text] [Medline]
  58. Lanca C, Saw S. The association between digital screen time and myopia: A systematic review. Ophthalmic Physiol Opt 2020 Mar 13;40(2):216-229. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  59. Morgan IG, Rose KA. Myopia: is the nature-nurture debate finally over? Clin Exp Optom 2019 Jan;102(1):3-17. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  60. Hansen MH, Laigaard PP, Olsen EM, Skovgaard AM, Larsen M, Kessel L, et al. Low physical activity and higher use of screen devices are associated with myopia at the age of 16-17 years in the CCC2000 Eye Study. Acta Ophthalmol 2020 May;98(3):315-321. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  61. Gopinath B, Hardy LL, Baur LA, Teber E, Mitchell P. Influence of parental history of hypertension on screen time and physical activity in young offspring. J Hypertens 2012 Feb;30(2):336-341. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  62. Harrington SC, Stack J, O'Dwyer V. Risk factors associated with myopia in schoolchildren in Ireland. Br J Ophthalmol 2019 Dec;103(12):1803-1809. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  63. Yang G, Huang L, Schmid KL, Li C, Chen J, He G, et al. Associations Between Screen Exposure in Early Life and Myopia amongst Chinese Preschoolers. Int J Environ Res Public Health 2020 Feb 07;17(3):1056 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  64. Toh SH, Coenen P, Howie EK, Smith AJ, Mukherjee S, Mackey DA, et al. A prospective longitudinal study of mobile touch screen device use and musculoskeletal symptoms and visual health in adolescents. Appl Ergon 2020 May;85:103028. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  65. Enthoven CA, Tideman JWL, Polling JR, Yang-Huang J, Raat H, Klaver CCW. The impact of computer use on myopia development in childhood: The Generation R study. Prev Med 2020 Mar;132:105988 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  66. Jones-Jordan LA, Mitchell GL, Cotter SA, Kleinstein RN, Manny RE, Mutti DO, CLEERE Study Group. Visual activity before and after the onset of juvenile myopia. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci 2011 Mar;52(3):1841-1850 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  67. Wu P, Tsai C, Wu H, Yang Y, Kuo H. Outdoor activity during class recess reduces myopia onset and progression in school children. Ophthalmology 2013 May;120(5):1080-1085. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  68. Ramamurthy D, Lin Chua SY, Saw S. A review of environmental risk factors for myopia during early life, childhood and adolescence. Clin Exp Optom 2015 Nov;98(6):497-506. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  69. Dirani M, Crowston JG, Wong TY. From reading books to increased smart device screen time. Br J Ophthalmol 2019 Jan;103(1):1-2. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  70. McCloskey M, Johnson SL, Benz C, Thompson DA, Chamberlin B, Clark L, et al. Parent Perceptions of Mobile Device Use Among Preschool-Aged Children in Rural Head Start Centers. J Nutr Educ Behav 2018 Jan;50(1):83-89. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  71. Wilkie HJ, Standage M, Gillison FB, Cumming SP, Katzmarzyk PT. The home electronic media environment and parental safety concerns: relationships with outdoor time after school and over the weekend among 9-11 year old children. BMC Public Health 2018 Apr 05;18(1):456 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  72. Hardell L, Sage C. Biological effects from electromagnetic field exposure and public exposure standards. Biomed Pharmacother 2008 Feb;62(2):104-109. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  73. Kheifets L, Repacholi M, Saunders R, van Deventer E. The sensitivity of children to electromagnetic fields. Pediatrics 2005 Aug;116(2):e303-e313. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  74. Balci M, Devrim E, Durak I. Effects of mobile phones on oxidant/antioxidant balance in cornea and lens of rats. Curr Eye Res 2007 Jan;32(1):21-25. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  75. Akar A, Karayiğit MO, Bolat D, Gültiken ME, Yarim M, Castellani G. Effects of low level electromagnetic field exposure at 2.45 GHz on rat cornea. Int J Radiat Biol 2013 Apr;89(4):243-249. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  76. Lixia S, Yao K, Kaijun W, Deqiang L, Huajun H, Xiangwei G, et al. Effects of 1.8 GHz radiofrequency field on DNA damage and expression of heat shock protein 70 in human lens epithelial cells. Mutat Res 2006 Dec 01;602(1-2):135-142. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  77. Leitgeb N. Mobile phones: are children at higher risk? Wien Med Wochenschr 2008;158(1-2):36-41. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  78. Khurana VG, Teo C, Kundi M, Hardell L, Carlberg M. Cell phones and brain tumors: a review including the long-term epidemiologic data. Surg Neurol 2009 Sep;72(3):205-214; discussion 214. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  79. Elder JA. Ocular effects of radiofrequency energy. Bioelectromagnetics 2003;Suppl 6:S148-S161. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  80. Yao K, Wang KJ, Sun ZH, Tan J, Xu W, Zhu LJ, et al. Low power microwave radiation inhibits the proliferation of rabbit lens epithelial cells by upregulating P27Kip1 expression. Mol Vis 2004 Feb 25;10:138-143 [FREE Full text] [Medline]
  81. Challis LJ. Mechanisms for interaction between RF fields and biological tissue. Bioelectromagnetics 2005;Suppl 7:S98-S106. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  82. Jones LA, Sinnott LT, Mutti DO, Mitchell GL, Moeschberger ML, Zadnik K. Parental history of myopia, sports and outdoor activities, and future myopia. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci 2007 Aug;48(8):3524-3532 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  83. Rose KA, Morgan IG, Ip J, Kifley A, Huynh S, Smith W, et al. Outdoor activity reduces the prevalence of myopia in children. Ophthalmology 2008 Aug;115(8):1279-1285. [CrossRef] [Medline]


EMF: electromagnetic field
EMR: electromagnetic radiation
ES: effect size
JBI: Joanna Briggs Institute
LOO: leave-one-out
OR: odds ratio
RF: radiofrequency


Edited by G Eysenbach; submitted 29.06.20; peer-reviewed by K Turner, T Igbe; comments to author 26.07.20; accepted 29.10.20; published 08.12.20

Copyright

©Jian Wang, Mei Li, Daqiao Zhu, Yang Cao. Originally published in the Journal of Medical Internet Research (http://www.jmir.org), 08.12.2020.

This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work, first published in the Journal of Medical Internet Research, is properly cited. The complete bibliographic information, a link to the original publication on http://www.jmir.org/, as well as this copyright and license information must be included.