Published on in Vol 19, No 12 (2017): December

Preprints (earlier versions) of this paper are available at https://preprints.jmir.org/preprint/8847, first published .
Individual Differences in the Relationship Between Attachment and Nomophobia Among College Students: The Mediating Role of Mindfulness

Individual Differences in the Relationship Between Attachment and Nomophobia Among College Students: The Mediating Role of Mindfulness

Individual Differences in the Relationship Between Attachment and Nomophobia Among College Students: The Mediating Role of Mindfulness

Original Paper

1Department of Computers and Instructional Technologies, Faculty of Education, Gaziosmanpasa University, Tokat, Turkey

2Department of Special Education, Faculty of Education, Hacettepe University, Ankara, Turkey

3Department of Counseling, Ahmet Kelesoglu Faculty of Education, Necmettin Erbakan University, Konya, Turkey

Corresponding Author:

Mustafa Baloğlu, PhD

Department of Special Education

Faculty of Education

Hacettepe University

Beytepe Campus

Ankara, 06110

Turkey

Phone: 90 3127806315

Fax:90 3127806314

Email: baloglu@hotmail.com


Background: There is a growing interest in nomophobia, which is defined as the fear of being out of cellular phone contact, or "feelings of discomfort or anxiety experienced by individuals when they are unable to use their mobile phones or utilize the affordances these devices provide”. However, only limited research can be found in terms of its determinants at present. Contemporary literature suggests that the relationships among attachment styles, mindfulness, and nomophobia have not been investigated.

Objective: This study aims to investigate the mediating effect of mindfulness on the relationship between attachment and nomophobia. In addition, the study also focuses on gender differences in attachment, mindfulness, and nomophobia. A theory-based structural model was tested to understand the essentials of the associations between the constructs.

Methods: The Experiences in Close Relationships Scale, Nomophobia Questionnaire, and Mindful Attention Awareness Scale were used to collect data from undergraduate students (N=450; 70.9% women [319/450]; mean age=21.94 years [SD 3.61]). Two measurement models (ie, attachment and mindfulness) and a structural model were specified, estimated, and evaluated.

Results: The structural equation model shows that the positive direct effects of avoidant (.13, P=.03) and anxious attachment (.48, P<.001) on nomophobia were significant. The negative direct effects of avoidant (−.18, P=.01) and anxious attachment (−.33, P<.001) on mindfulness were also significant. Moreover, mindfulness has a significant negative effect on nomophobia for women only (−.13, P=.03). Finally, the Sobel test showed that the indirect effects of avoidant and anxious attachment on nomophobia via mindfulness were significant (P<.001). The direct and indirect effects of anxious attachment, avoidant attachment, and mindfulness altogether accounted for 33% of the total variance in nomophobia. Gender comparison results show that there is a significant difference in attachment based on gender (F2,447=6.97, P=.01, Wilk λ=.97, partial η2=.03). Women (mean 68.46 [SD 16.96]) scored significantly higher than men (mean 63.59 [SD 15.97]) in anxious attachment (F1=7.93, P=.01, partial η2=.02). Gender differences in mindfulness were not significant (F4,448=3.45, P=.69). On the other hand, results do show significant gender differences in nomophobia (F4,445=2.71, P=.03, Wilk λ=.98, partial η2=.02) where women scored significantly higher than men.

Conclusions: In general, individuals who are emotionally more dependent and crave more closeness and attention in the relationship tend to display higher levels of fear or discomfort when they have no access to their mobile phones. However, gender has a differential impact on the relationship between avoidant attachment and nomophobia. This study establishes the impact of mindfulness on nomophobia for women; therefore, future studies should test the effectiveness of mindfulness-based therapy approaches and confirm whether they are effective and efficient. On the basis of significant gender difference in nomophobia and attachment, we conclude that gender should be taken into account in mindfulness-based treatments dealing with nomophobia.

J Med Internet Res 2017;19(12):e404

doi:10.2196/jmir.8847

Keywords



Nomophobia

Excessive or problematic technology use has been partly explained by attachment theory, which posits that people may cultivate connection to technological devices and may feel uncomfortable upon separation [1]. Among various types of problematic technology use, nomophobia is a relatively novel problem [2,3] or disorder [4], which is defined as the fear of being unable to use one’s mobile device [5] or the “the feelings of discomfort or anxiety experienced by individuals when they are unable to use their mobile phones or utilize the affordances these devices provide” [6]. Lin et al [7] categorize nomophobia as a form of situational phobia and suggest its inclusion into the specific phobia in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition (DSM-V) as a situational phobia [8,9]. Han et al [10] defined nomophobia as a smartphone separation anxiety and argued that when a user perceives the smartphone as an extended self, he or she is more likely to get attached to the device, which in turn would lead to nomophobia by promoting proximity seeking.

Although there is a growing interest in nomophobia, at present, limited research can be found in terms of its determinants. Nonetheless, some of the technology-related situational, dispositional, and environmental antecedents that may share the similar fundamentals with nomophobia are summarized in Table 1. This review indicates that there is a common pattern of dispositional (eg, impulsivity, self-control, self-esteem, or introversion), situational (eg, age, gender, marital status, or ethnicity), and environmental (eg, Web-based social support, amount of time, or stress) antecedents that are associated with technology-related complications.

Attachment

Attachment styles may be one of the dispositional antecedents of nomophobia, where attachment is defined as “the over-allocation of cognitive and emotional resources towards a particular object, construct, or idea” [21]. Attachment refers to “the mental representations of the self, partner, and relationships called internal working models,” which incorporate self-perceptions of one’s personal worth of love and care from others and of others’ availability and awareness to one’s needs [22]. Attachment theory has originally hypothesized the predisposition of humans to form and preserve strong emotional bonds with their caregivers [23]. However, later, the likelihood of emotional bondage beyond humans has been suggested, in the forms of obsessive-compulsive disorder [24], compulsive work [25], or various types of addictions [26-28]. Thus, Van Gordon et al [29] conclude that unhealthy attachments to objects, people, or situations may be developed.

The relationship between attachment styles and substance addictions has been documented in the literature [30,31]. The mechanisms underlying substance addictions may be similar for behavioral complications such as addictions or phobias in terms of technology, Internet, or social media. For example, anonymous and interactive communication in social media can reduce the feeling of social isolation for individuals with an insecure attachment [32]. Recently, Eichenberg et al [30] reported that individuals with a tendency for Internet addiction were categorized as insecurely attached, whereas securely attached individuals showed no tendency for Internet addiction.

Table 1. Selected situational, dispositional, and environmental antecedents of technology-related complications.
StudyDomainAntecedents
Arpaci et al [11]Internet addictionIndividualism and psychological needs (ie, achievement, affiliation, dominance, and autonomy)
Karacic and Oreskovic [12]Internet addictionAge and gender
Lee et al [13]Smartphone addictionSelf-expressive and utilitarian benefits and locus of control
Kim and Kang [14]Mobile messenger services addictionSelf-esteem and self-control
Shen et al [15]Web-based game addictionLoneliness, achievement, and excitement
Chen et al [16]Smartphone addictionPerceived enjoyment, mood regulation, pastime, and conformity
Jeong et al [17]Digital game addictionLoneliness, depression, and aggression
Yang et al [18]Mobile social networking sites addictionWeb-based social interaction ties and Web-based social supports
Bock et al [19]Mobile phone affinityAnxious attachment and addiction correlates with depression and impulsivity
Kim et al [20]Smartphone addictionImpulsivity

Studies such as these suggest that attachment theory may help us understand the antecedents of technology-relevant behavioral complications such as nomophobia.

Mindfulness is described as “the awareness that emerges through paying non-judgmental attention on purpose, in the present moment, to the unfolding of experience moment by moment” [33]. Mindfulness is found to have positive effects on physical and psychological well-being [34]. In addition, mindfulness has been theorized to have effects on behavioral addictions, depression, mood disorder, and anxiety disorder [34,35]. Consequently, studies have found mindfulness mediation interventions to improve a variety of health conditions [36].

In sum, an evaluation of the contemporary literature suggests that the relationships among attachment styles and problematic technology use have not been fully investigated [37]. An investigation of the effects of various attachment styles (ie, avoidant attachment vs anxious attachment) on nomophobia would be highly original in the literature. Furthermore, studying the potential effects of mindfulness on the association between attachment and nomophobia would be interesting and theoretically warranted. Lastly, because gender roles theory posits that there exist cognitive and emotional differences between men and women, this research aimed to investigate the effects of gender differences on attachment, mindfulness, and nomophobia.

Theoretical Background and Hypothesis

Attachment Theory

Bowlby’s [22,38-43] classical research on the infant-mother attachment theory has been extended to different populations [36,37], relationship types [38,39], and nonhuman objects [28,44]. Hazan and Shaver [45] classified attachment styles into three general categories, that is, secure, anxious, and avoidant, which are consistent with the three childhood attachment styles suggested in the original studies with infants.

Secure attachment is associated with a positive model of self as well as a positive model of others. Therefore, individuals who score lower on both dimensions are classified as insecurely attached [46]. Avoidant attachment is associated with a positive model of self and a negative model of others. It can be construed as hypersensitivity to unresponsiveness, rejection, and abandonment. Individuals whose dominant style is avoidant tend to have difficulties with close relationships and intimacy and seek to maintain self-reliance, emotional distance, and control [45]. They distance themselves from others and avoid experiencing negative emotions [47].

Anxious attachment is associated with a negative model of self and a positive model of others [45,46]. Anxious attachment is characterized by greater emotional dependence, desire for more commitment and closeness [48], and more intensive attention from partners [47]. Individuals dominant in this attachment style tend to have a fear of rejection, a negative perception of self-worth, worry about being abandoned or unloved, and thereby, seek a higher need for closeness and intimacy [49]. They seem to be obsessive and hypervigilant in the relationship [50]. This study opted to focus on the effects of anxious versus avoidant attachment styles on nomophobia and mindfulness.

Attachment styles have an impact on social interactions and emotional development [51,52], which in turn determine the risk of developing dependence to people, objects, or events [26,53]. Recent studies indicate a significant relationship between attachment styles (ie, avoidant vs anxious) and behavioral addictions [26-28]. In addition, attachment styles are related to the problematic use of technology such as the Internet [54], mobile phones [28], video games [55], and social media [26]. More specifically, anxious attachment was associated with technology-mediated breakups, whereas avoidant attachment predicted the likelihood of technology overuse [56]. Blackwell et al [26] found that both attachment styles predict social media addiction. On the basis of attachment theory and available research findings, we hypothesized that the effects of both anxious attachment (hypothesis 1) and avoidant attachment (hypothesis 2) on nomophobia would be positively significant.

Mindfulness

Mindfulness-based mental health betterment techniques assert that behavioral tendencies that lead to psychological complications can distort individuals’ perceptions of reality and may cause maladjustment [57]. Accordingly, the American Psychiatric Association [58] promotes mindfulness-based therapy approaches, which have been found effective in the treatment of behavioral addictions, depression, mood disorder, and anxiety disorder [35,59].

There is promising evidence for the efficacy of mindfulness-based treatment, suggesting that self-awareness increased through mindfulness practices can target multiple neural, psychological, physiological, and behavioral processes [34,35,60-63]. In the same vein, recent studies argued that mindfulness-based interventions could treat behavioral complications such as Internet addiction [64] and video game addiction [65]. Problematic technology use, such as nomophobia, may also be treated by interventions that gear toward enhancing individuals’ mindfulness levels [66]. Therefore, we hypothesized that higher levels of mindfulness would be associated with lower levels of nomophobia. In other words, there would be a negatively significant effect of mindfulness on nomophobia (hypothesis 3).

There is strong evidence that mindfulness is associated with attachment styles. For example, Pepping et al [67] indicated that avoidant and anxious attachment were significantly associated with lower scores in mindfulness. In another study, mindfulness was negatively linked with anxious attachment [68]. Furthermore, studies have validated the association between mindfulness and attachment styles [69-71]. On the other hand, several studies argued that mindfulness is significantly associated with attachment security [67,72]. Thus, we hypothesized that there would be a significant negative effect of both anxious attachment (hypothesis 4) and avoidant attachment (hypothesis 5) on mindfulness. Finally, based on gender roles theory, we expected significant gender differences between men and women in attachment, mindfulness, and nomophobia.


Participants

A total of 450 students were recruited through convenience sampling method. Of the 450 students, 319 were women (70.9%) and 131 were men (29.1%). Participants’ ages ranged from 18 to 40 years (mean age=21.94 [SD 3.61]). In terms of ownership, 99.3% of the participants had a smartphone (447/450), whereas 95.6% (430/450) had mobile Internet. Participants used mobile Internet on an average of 4.58 hours a day (SD 2.92). In terms of college levels, 123 students were freshmen (27.3%); 11 were sophomores (2.4%); 153 were juniors (34.0%); and 163 were seniors (36.3%). Participants came from various study majors such as Psychology (41.3%), Social Science (33%), Health Science (17.1%), and Computer Science (8.6%).

Measures

The Experiences in Close Relationships (ECR) Scale

Brennan et al [46] developed the ECR scale, a 36-item, self-report attachment measure, which includes 2 subscales: Avoidance (alpha=.94) and Anxiety (alpha=.91). Sample items from the scale include “I prefer not to show a partner how I feel deep down” (ie, Avoidance) and “worry about being abandoned” (ie, Anxiety). Items are rated on a 7-point Likert scale, ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree), and higher scores refer to higher levels of avoidance or anxiety. Sümer [73] provided evidence for the reliability and validity of the Turkish version of the ECR scale. Cronbach alpha coefficients for anxiety and avoidance styles were found to be .86 and .90, respectively [73]. In this study, the instrument’s internal consistency coefficients for the combined groups, men, and women were found to be acceptable (Table 2).

The Mindful Attention Awareness Scale (MAAS)

Brown and Ryan [74] developed 15 items rated on a 6-point Likert scale that form a single factor structure to assess the awareness of the present moment and the level of attention. Sample items from the scale include “I could be experiencing some emotion and not be conscious of it until sometime later” and “I find it difficult to stay focused on what’s happening in the present.” Özyeşil et al [75] adapted the scale into Turkish and provided validity and reliability properties of the adapted scale. The scale’s internal consistency coefficient was found to be .85 in this study (Table 2).

The Nomophobia Questionnaire (NMP-Q)

The NMP-Q is a 20-item, 5-point Likert type, self-report instrument that assesses the fear of being unable to use mobile devices under 4 subscales (ie, Unable to Access Information, Losing Connectedness, Unable to Communicate, and Giving Up Convenience). Sample items from the questionnaire include the following: “I would be annoyed if I could not look information up on my smartphone when I wanted to do so” (ie, Unable to Access Information), “If I could not use my smartphone, I would be afraid of getting stranded somewhere” (ie, Losing Connectedness), “I would be anxious because I could not keep in touch with my family and/or friends” (ie, Unable to Communicate), or “I would feel awkward because I could not check my notifications for updates from my connections and Web-based networks” (ie, Giving Up Convenience) [5]. Cronbach alpha of the original scale was .92. Yildirim et al [76] provide the validity and reliability evidence of the Turkish NMP-Q. They found that Cronbach alpha coefficients of the 4 subscales were .90, .74, .94, and .91, respectively. We used the total nomophobia scores in this study, which are computed by summing the 4 subscale scores. The internal consistency coefficients of the questionnaire ranged from .89 to .92 for the combined groups, men, and women in this study (Table 2).

Procedure

All procedures were performed in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional board guidelines, and the study was approved by the institutional review board. Before informed consent was obtained, participants were debriefed about the nature and possible consequences of the study. Participation was voluntary and participants received extra course credit for completing the research packet.

Table 2. Descriptive statistics, correlation coefficients, convergent validity and discriminant validity, and reliability of the study variables. Statistics reported outside parentheses are for the combined groups, whereas within parentheses are statistics for men and women, respectively.
VariablesAlphaCRaAVEbCorrelations




1234
1. Anxious.88 (.87-.89).77 (.80-.76).52 (.50-.50).72 (.71-.71)c


2. Avoidant.77 (.75-.77).84 (.83-.83).57 (.55-.55).09 (.18-.10).75 (.74-.74)

3. Mindfulness.85 (.87-.85).79 (.70-.78).50 (.50-.51)−.26 (−.24 to −.25)−.22 (−.42 to −.15).71 (.71-.71)

4. Nomophobia.91 (.89-.92).75 (.81-.77).50 (.50-.50).54 (.47-.56).27 (.23-.28)−.35 (−.35 to −.35).71 (.71-.71)
Range1-71-71-61-5
Mean4.31 (4.47-4.24)3.82 (3.55-3.93)3.77 (3.75-3.78)3.25 (3.22-3.26)
SDd1.75 (1.70-1.75)1.73 (1.63-1.74)1.33 (1.38-1.31)1.10 (1.11-1.09)

aCR: composite reliability.

bAVE: average variance extracted (for convergent validity).

cDiscriminant validity coefficients are on the diagonal.

dSD: standard deviation.


Instrument Reliability and Validity

The normality of the scales suggested minimal skewness (range −.34 to .33) and kurtosis (range −.50 to 2.11). Convergent and discriminant validity of the constructs were investigated by developing a correlation matrix. Hair et al [77] suggest that convergent validity is adequate when average variance extracted (AVE) coefficients exceed or equal .50. In this study, the square roots of the AVE coefficients (shown in diagonal Table 2) were greater than the interconstruct correlations for all constructs. Thus, discriminant validity was found satisfactory for the constructs studied. Table 2 also illustrates reliability and convergent validity coefficients along with descriptive statistics for the combined groups, women, and men.

Gender Differences

One-way multivariate analysis of variances investigated statistical differences between men (n=131) and women (n=319) in attachment, mindfulness, and nomophobia. Results show that there is a significant difference in attachment based on gender (F2,447=6.97, P=.01, Wilk λ=.97, partial η2=.03). Women (mean 68.46 [SD 16.96]) scored significantly higher than men (mean 63.59 [SD 15.97]) in anxious attachment (F1=7.93, P=.01, partial η2=.017). Gender differences in mindfulness were not statistically significant (F4,448=3.45, P=.69). On the other hand, results show significant gender differences in nomophobia (F4,445=2.71, P=.03, Wilk λ=.98, partial η2=.02), where women scored significantly higher than men.

Confirmatory Factor Analysis

Confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) was used to test whether the proposed measurement models and the structural model were to fit the data. The model fit indices for the measurement models and the structural model are presented in Table 3. Results suggest that the measures used in this study formed adequate measurement models and therefore, provided evidence for the construct validity.

Hypothesis Testing

To test the research model and hypotheses, structural equation modeling (SEM) was employed with maximum likelihood estimation. The SEM results for the combined groups, men, and women are presented in Table 4. As shown in the table, the positive direct effect of anxious attachment was significant on nomophobia for the combined groups, women, and men; however, the direct effect of avoidant attachment on nomophobia was significant only for the combined groups but not for men or women. Therefore, results shown in Figure 1 supported hypothesis 1 but not hypothesis 2. Moreover, there is a significant negative effect of mindfulness on nomophobia for the combined groups (−.13, t419=−2.16, R2=.15, P=.03) and women (−.15, t318=−2.23, R2=.15, P=.03) but not for men (−.13, t130=−1.12, P=.26). Thus, results supported hypothesis 3 for the combined groups and women but not for men.

Table 3. Fit indices of the measurement models and the theoretical model. The results of multigroup analysis are shown in parentheses.
Fit indicesAttachmentNomophobiaStructural modelAcceptable values
Chi-square83.51 (134.12)401.75 (574.95)333.14 (612.78)
Degrees of freedom33 (66)156 (310)216 (432)
P value.001 (.001).001 (.001).001 (.001).05≤ P ≤1.00 [79]
Chi-square/degrees of freedom2.53 (2.03)2.58 (1.86)1.54 (1.42)<3 [78
GFIa.97 (.94).92 (.89).94 (.90)≥.90 [78,79]
AGFIb.94 (.91).89 (.86).92 (.87)≥.80 [78,79]
RMRc.03 (.03).06 (.07).02 (.03)<.05 [78,79]
RMSEAd.06 (05).06 (.04).04 (.03)<.08 [77]
NFIe.94 (.90).92 (.89).90 (.81)≥.90 [77]
TLIf.95 (.93).93 (.93).95 (.92)≥.90 [78,79]
CFIg.96 (.95).95 (.94).96 (.94)≥.90 [78,79]
IFIh.96 (.95).95 (.94).96 (.94)≥.90 [78,79]

aGFI: Goodness-of-Fit Index.

bAGFI: Adjusted Goodness-of-Fit Index.

cRMR : root mean square residual.

dRMSEA: root mean square error of approximation.

eNFI: Normed Fit Index.

fTLI: Tucker-Lewis Index.

gCFI: Comparative Fit Index.

hIFI: Incremental Fit Index.

Table 4. Structural equation modeling (SEM) and multigroup analysis results. Results are reported for the combined groups (men, women), respectively.

Hypothesis

Estimate
SEa
Critical ratio

P value
Anxious → Nomophobia.48 (.53-.46).17 (.42-.18)6.93 (3.53-5.82).001 (.001-.001)
Avoidant → Nomophobia.13 (.02-.12).30 (.37-.29)2.21 (.18-1.81).03 (.86-.07)
Mindfulness → Nomophobia−.13 (−.13 to −.15).35 (.63-.44)−2.16 (−1.12 to −2.23).03 (.26-.03)
Anxious → Mindfulness−.33 (−.26 to −.31).03 (.06-.03)−4.66 (−2.19 to −3.75).001 (.03-.001)
Avoidant → Mindfulness−.18 (−.33 to −.15).06 (.09-.05)−2.63 (−2.28 to −2.05).01 (.02-.04)

aSE: standard error.

Figure 1. Hypothesis testing results. Manifest variables and their respected error terms underlying the latent variables are omitted to save space. "a" signifies P<.001.
View this figure

Furthermore, negative direct effects of both anxious and avoidant attachment on mindfulness were significant for the combined groups, men, and women. Therefore, results supported both hypothesis 4 and hypothesis 5. The direct and indirect effects of anxious attachment, avoidant attachment, and mindfulness altogether accounted for 33% of the total variance in nomophobia. The equations shown in Table 5 represent the relationships among the constructs.

Mediation Analysis

A 4-step approach was used to test the mediation effect of mindfulness on the relationship between attachment and nomophobia. First, the direct effects of both anxious (SE=.13, t=−3.19, P<.001) and avoidant (SE=.18, t=−3.54, P<.001) attachment on mindfulness were found significant. Second, both the direct effects of anxious (SE=.41, t=4.02, P<.001) and avoidant (SE=.39, t=2.11, P=.05) attachment on nomophobia were found significant as well. Third, the direct effect of mindfulness on nomophobia was significant (SE=.65, t=−3.50, P<.001). Finally, the Sobel test showed that the indirect effect of avoidant and anxious attachment on nomophobia via the mediator (ie, mindfulness) was significant (P<.001). These results supported full mediation and indicated that attachments have significant effects on nomophobia through mindfulness.

Randomization Tests

Because we used a nonrandom sample, randomization tests were conducted to support generalizability beyond this study’s sample. We employed 5000 bootstrap replicates to test the effects of attachment on nomophobia via mindfulness. Means, standard errors, 95% CIs, significance levels, and the directions of the relations are reported in Figure 2, which altogether suggest that similar results closely approximated in the bootstrapped samples.

Table 5. Relationships among the constructs. Results are reported for the combined groups (men, women), respectively.

Variable

Equation

Error
R2
Mindfulness−.18 (−.33 to −.15) x Avoidant −.33 (−.26 to −.31) x Anxious.13 (.15-.13).15 (.24-.12)
Nomophobia.13 (.02-.12) x Avoidant .48 (.53-.46) x Anxious −.13 (−.13 to −.15) x Mindfulness3.83 (3.55-3.94).33 (.36-.31)
Figure 2. Means of 5000 bootstrap replicate coefficients and their standard errors. Within parentheses are 95% CIs. Manifest variables and their respected error terms underlying the latent variables are omitted to save space. "a" signifies P=.05 and "b" signifies P<.001.
View this figure

Principal Findings

One way of explaining why people experience problems in technology use is that of attachment theory. Recent research has found significant associations among different attachment styles and problematic use of the Internet, mobile phones, video games, and social media [26-28]. Therefore, the aim of this study was to further investigate the effects of anxious and avoidant attachment on nomophobia. Additionally, because the concept of mindfulness has been theorized to have plausible effects on psychological complications, we aimed to test whether it would mediate the association between attachment and nomophobia.

In general, we found positive effects of both anxious and avoidant attachment on nomophobia and their negative effects on mindfulness. However, when we analyzed the details of the effects for men and women separately, results revealed interesting patterns. For example, anxious attachment had a significant positive effect on nomophobia for both women and men, even though the link was stronger among men than women. Individuals who are emotionally more dependent and crave for more closeness and attention in the relationship tend to display higher levels of fear or discomfort when they have no access to their mobile phones. Thus, we conclude that anxious attachment is reflected upon a technological object, that is, a smartphone. These findings suggest the likelihood of a strong link between anxious attachment and nomophobia, which should be further scrutinized through experimental studies.

When it comes to avoidant attachment, results are not as straightforward. We found that avoidant attachment shows a significant positive effect on nomophobia on the combined groups (N=450); however, the effects reduced to nonsignificance when men (n=131) and women (n=319) were analyzed separately. This suggests that gender has a differential impact on the relation between avoidant attachment and nomophobia.

Literature suggest that mindfulness cultivates a better understanding of the nature of existence by mindfully observing the dissolution of affective, cognitive, and sensory processes [29]. In addition, mindfulness is used effectively in various behavioral dependencies such as Internet gaming disorder [35,80]. Lastly, it has been suggested that technology-based addictions may be treated by interventions that gear toward enhancing mindfulness [34,35,61,81]. This study is the first attempt in the literature to investigate the effect of mindfulness on nomophobia. Results indicate a significant direct effect of mindfulness on nomophobia for women but not for men. Women who show higher levels of mindfulness carry lower risk of nomophobia. Similar to our results, Sriwilai and Charoensukmongkol [37] examined the relationship between social media addiction and mindfulness. They found that individuals with higher social media addiction have lower mindfulness scores. These findings indicate that mindfulness-based therapies could be used in the treatment of behavioral addictions for women. Similar suggestions were made by Wahbeh and Oken [36], Garland et al [82], Black [83], Peltz and Black [84], and Garland [85].

Prior studies suggested that men and women show quite different Web-based behavior patterns. For example, Durkee et al [86] indicated that male adolescents prefer to use Web-based games, whereas female adolescents prefer to interact with social networking websites. Our findings broaden the literature by suggesting the possibility of using mindfulness-based treatment techniques with nomophobia for women. There are two general implications of these findings. First, the concept and techniques of mindfulness can be used as a preventive tool to dodge the risk of nomophobia for women. Once risk assessments are completed and under-risk groups are identified, mindfulness exercises may be taught and practiced with under-risk women. Second, mindfulness-based treatment techniques may be used with clinically diagnosed women to remedy the higher levels of nomophobia. This study establishes the impact of mindfulness on nomophobia at least for women; therefore, future studies should test the effectiveness of mindfulness-based therapy approaches and confirm whether they are effective and efficient.

Findings indicate that there is a significant difference in nomophobia between women and men. Furthermore, there was a significant difference in the attachment styles based on gender. Results indicate that women and men differed in anxious attachment but not in avoidant attachment. These findings imply that women tend to display more anxious attachment than men. Gender differences in attachment can be explained by gender schema theory proposed by Bem [87]. She theorizes that individuals develop cognitive schemas throughout childhood to be able to show gender-appropriate behaviors. Such schemas eventually predispose men and women to form and preserve emotional bonds differently. Previously, Katz and Toner [88] conducted a systematic review to figure out the role of gender differences in the effectiveness of the mindfulness-based treatment for substance use. Their findings suggested that men gravitate less toward the treatments, and thereby, women benefit more from such treatments. These findings imply that gender differences should be considered in mindfulness-based treatments dealing with nomophobia.

Limitations

There are several limitations of this study. First, using a convenience sampling in recruiting the participants is one of the limitations of the study. We attempted to overcome this limitation by conducting randomization tests; however, future studies may analyze the hypothesized relationships on longitudinal data or random samples. Second, the study shows an overwhelming use of female subjects as compared with male subjects; the sample size for men is quite limited to test SEM-based analyses, and we suggest that the models established in this study be tested with a larger sample, which is fairly well balanced by gender. Third, neither is the attachment style the only determinant of nomophobia nor is mindfulness the only mediator; however, based on cyber psychology literature, the study focused on these factors. Other dispositional or situational factors (ie, personality characteristics) and mediators (ie, psychological needs) would definitely be worthwhile to explore in future studies. Finally, it would be useful to incorporate qualitative techniques for a detailed investigation of the research problem. Despite these limitations, this study has significant contributions to expand the literature by identifying important relationships among attachment, mindfulness, and nomophobia.

Conclusions

We found the significant effects of both anxious and avoidant attachment on mindfulness and nomophobia. However, gender has a differential impact on the relation between avoidant attachment and nomophobia. We conclude that gender differences should be considered in mindfulness-based treatments dealing with nomophobia. Mindfulness-based therapies could be used in the treatment of nomophobia, especially for women.

Conflicts of Interest

None declared.

  1. Seol J. San Francisco, CA: San Francisco State University; 2016. Self-disclosure in American friendships: links with collectivism and adult attachment styles   URL: https://sfsu-dspace.calstate.edu/bitstream/handle/10211.3/181604/AS362016PSYCHS46.pdf?sequence=1 [accessed 2017-11-15] [WebCite Cache]
  2. King AL, Valença AM, Silva AC, Sancassiani F, Machado S, Nardi AE. “Nomophobia”: impact of cell phone use interfering with symptoms and emotions of individuals with panic disorder compared with a control group. Clin Pract Epidemiol Ment Health 2014;10:28-35. [CrossRef]
  3. Dixit S, Shukla H, Bhagwat A, Bindal A, Goyal A, Zaidi AK, et al. A study to evaluate mobile phone dependence among students of a medical college and associated hospital of central India. Indian J Community Med 2010 Apr;35(2):339-341 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  4. King AL, Valença AM, Nardi AE. Nomophobia: the mobile phone in panic disorder with agoraphobia: reducing phobias or worsening of dependence? Cogn Behav Neurol 2010 Mar;23(1):52-54. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  5. Yildirim C, Correia AP. Exploring the dimensions of nomophobia: development and validation of a self-reported questionnaire. Comput Human Behav 2015;49:130-137. [CrossRef]
  6. King AL, Valença AM, Silva AC, Baczynski T, Carvalho MR, Nardi AE. Nomophobia: dependency on virtual environments or social phobia? Comput Human Behav 2013;29(1):140-144. [CrossRef]
  7. Lin YH, Chang LR, Lee YH, Tseng HW, Kuo TB, Chen SH. Development and validation of the Smartphone Addiction Inventory (SPAI). PLoS One 2014 Jun 4;9(6):e98312 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  8. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 5th Edition. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association Publishing; 2013.
  9. Bragazzi NL, Del Puente G. A proposal for including nomophobia in the new DSM-V. Psychol Res Behav Manag 2014 May 16;7:155-160. [Medline]
  10. Han S, Kim KJ, Kim JH. Understanding nomophobia: structural equation modeling and semantic network analysis of smartphone separation anxiety. Cyberpsychol Behav Soc Netw 2017 Jul;20(7):419-427. [Medline]
  11. Arpaci I, Kesici Ş, Baloğlu M. Individualism and Internet addiction: the mediating role of psychological needs. Internet Res 2018;27(5):Indexed in SSCI.
  12. Karacic S, Oreskovic S. Internet addiction through the phase of adolescence: a questionnaire study. JMIR Mental Health 2017 May 3;4(2):e11 [FREE Full text] [Medline]
  13. Lee Y, Chang C, Lin Y, Cheng Z. Heads-down tribes across four Asian countries: antecedents of smartphone addiction. Int J Mobile Comm 2017 Jan;15(4):414-436. [CrossRef]
  14. Kim B, Kang M. Effect of MMS addiction on user's health and academic performance in an era of convergence. J Digit Conver 2016;14(1):131-139. [CrossRef]
  15. Shen X, Ng JCY, Tan X. An empirical study on the antecedents to college student online game addiction in China. Indian J Comm Manag Stud 2016;7(1):8-11 [FREE Full text]
  16. Chen C, Zhang KKZ, Gong X, Zhao SJ, Lee MKO, Liang L. Examining the effects of motives and gender differences on smartphone addiction. Comput Human Behav 2017;75:891-902. [CrossRef]
  17. Jeong EJ, Kim DJ, Lee DM. Why do some people become addicted to digital games more easily? A study of digital game addiction from a psychosocial health perspective. Int J Hum Comp Interact 2017;33(3):199-214. [CrossRef]
  18. Yang S, Liu Y, Wei J. Social capital on mobile SNS addiction: a perspective from online and offline channel integrations. Internet Res 2016;26(4):982-1000. [CrossRef]
  19. Bock BC, Lantini R, Thind H, Walaska K, Rosen RK, Fava JL, et al. The mobile phone affinity scale: enhancement and refinement. JMIR Mhealth Uhealth 2016;4(4):e134 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef]
  20. Kim SM, Huh HJ, Cho H, Kwon M, Choi JH, Ahn HJ, et al. The effect of depression, impulsivity, and resilience on smartphone addiction in university students. J Korean Neuropsychiatr Assoc 2014 Jul;53(4):214-220. [CrossRef]
  21. Shonin E, Van Gordon W, Griffiths MD. The emerging role of Buddhism in clinical psychology: toward effective integration. Psychol Relig Spirit 2014;6(2):123-137. [CrossRef]
  22. Bowlby J. Attachment and Loss: Vol. 1. Attachment. 2nd edition. New York, NY: Basic Books; 1982.
  23. Bowlby J. The making and breaking of affectional bonds. I. Aetiology and psychopathology in the light of attachment theory. An expanded version of the Fiftieth Maudsley Lecture, delivered before the Royal College of Psychiatrists, 19 November 1976. Br J Psychiatry 1977 Mar;130:201-210. [Medline]
  24. Yarbro J, Mahaffey B, Abramowitz J, Kashdan TB. Recollections of parent–child relationships, attachment insecurity, and obsessive–compulsive beliefs. Pers Individ Dif 2013 Feb;54(3):355-360. [CrossRef]
  25. Hazan C, Shaver PR. Love and work: an attachment-theoretical perspective. J Pers Soc Psychol 1990;59(2):270-280. [CrossRef]
  26. Blackwell D, Leaman C, Tramposch R, Osborne C, Liss M. Extraversion, neuroticism, attachment style and fear of missing out as predictors of social media use and addiction. Pers Individ Dif 2017 Oct 1;116:69-72. [CrossRef]
  27. Assunção RS, Costa P, Tagliabue S, Matos PM. Problematic Facebook use in adolescents: associations with parental attachment and alienation to peers. J Child Fam Stud 2017 Nov;26(11):2990-2998. [CrossRef]
  28. Fullwood C, Quinn S, Kaye LK, Redding C. My virtual friend: a qualitative analysis of the attitudes and experiences of smartphone users: implications for smartphone attachment. Comput Human Behav 2017 Oct;75:347-355. [CrossRef]
  29. Van Gordon W, Shonin E, Griffiths MD. Buddhist emptiness theory: implications for psychology. Psycholog Relig Spiritual 2016 Mar 17:1-30. [CrossRef]
  30. Eichenberg C, Schott M, Decker O, Sindelar B. Attachment style and internet addiction: an online survey. J Med Internet Res 2017 May 17;19(5):e170 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  31. Borhani Y. Substance abuse and insecure attachment styles: a relational study. LUX 2013;2(1):1-13. [CrossRef]
  32. Wildt B, Putzig I, Vukicevic A, Wedegärtner F. Störungen von selbsterleben und beziehungsverhalten bei menschen mit Internetabhängigkeit. Sucht 2011;57(1):17-26 [FREE Full text]
  33. Kabat-Zinn J. Wherever You Go, There You Are: Mindfulness Meditation in Everyday Life. New York, NY: Hyperion; 1994.
  34. Plaza I, Demarzo MM, Herrera-Mercadal P, Garcia-Campayo J. Mindfulness-based mobile applications: literature review and analysis of current features. JMIR Mhealth Uhealth 2013 Nov 1;1(2):e24 [FREE Full text] [Medline]
  35. Shonin E, Van Gordon W, Slade K, Griffiths MD. Mindfulness and other Buddhist-derived interventions in correctional settings: a systematic review. Aggress Violent Behav 2013;18(3):365-372. [CrossRef]
  36. Wahbeh H, Oken BS. Internet mindfulness meditation intervention for the general public: pilot randomized controlled trial. JMIR Ment Health 2016 Aug 8;3(3):e37 [FREE Full text] [Medline]
  37. Sriwilai K, Charoensukmongkol P. Face it, don't Facebook it: impacts of social media addiction on mindfulness, coping strategies and the consequence on emotional exhaustion. Stress Health 2016 Oct;32(4):427-434. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  38. Bowlby J. Attachment and Loss: Vol. 2. Separation: Anxiety and Anger. New York, NY: Basic Books; 1973.
  39. Bowlby J. Attachment and Loss: Vol. 3. Loss: Depression and Sadness. New York, NY: Basic Books; 1980.
  40. Jones JD, Ehrlich KB, Lejuez CW, Cassidy J. Parental knowledge of adolescent activities: links with parental attachment style and adolescent substance use. J Fam Psychol 2015 Apr;29(2):191-200 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  41. Morey JN, Gentzler AL, Creasy B, Oberhauser AM, Westerman D. Young adults’ use of communication technology within their romantic relationships and associations with attachment style. Comput Human Behav 2013 Jul;29(4):1771-1778. [CrossRef]
  42. Liu J, Wang Y, Jackson T. Towards explaining relationship dissatisfaction in Chinese dating couples: relationship disillusionment, emergent distress, or insecure attachment style? Pers Individ Dif 2017 Jul 1;112:42-48. [CrossRef]
  43. Wang H, Han Z. Transmission of attachment style and physiological stress response to romantic conflict in Chinese young couples: a dyadic analysis. 2016 Presented at: ICP 2016. The 31st International Congress of Psychology; July 27, 2016; Yokohama, Japan p. 726.
  44. Kim E, Cho I, Kim EJ. Structural equation model of smartphone addiction based on adult attachment theory: mediating effects of loneliness and depression. Asian Nurs Res (Korean Soc Nurs Sci) 2017 Jun;11(2):92-97 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  45. Hazan C, Shaver P. Romantic love conceptualized as an attachment process. J Pers Soc Psychol 1987 Mar;52(3):511-524. [Medline]
  46. Brennan KA, Clark CL, Shaver PR. Self-report measurement of adult romantic attachment: an integrative overview. In: Simpson JS, Rholes WS, editors. Attachment Theory and Close Relationships. New York, NY: Guilford Press; 1998:46-76.
  47. Pistole MC. College students' ended love relationships: attachment style and emotion. J Coll Stud Dev 1970;36(1):53-60.
  48. Feeney JA, Noller P. Attachment style as a predictor of adult romantic relationships. J Pers Soc Psychol 1990 Feb;58(2):281-291 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef]
  49. Mikulincer M, Shaver PR. Attachment in Adulthood: Structure, Dynamics, and Change. New York, NY: The Guilford University Press; 2007.
  50. Mikulincer M, Florian V, Tolmacz R. Attachment styles and fear of personal death: a case study of affect regulation. J Pers Soc Psychol 1990;58(2):273-280. [CrossRef]
  51. Sroufe LA, Egeland B, Kreutzer T. The fate of early experience following developmental change: longitudinal approaches to individual adaptation in childhood. Child Dev 1990;61:1363-1373. [CrossRef]
  52. Lamb ME. Infant-Mother Attachment: The Origins and Developmental Significance of Individual Differences in Strange Situation Behavior. Hillsdale, NJ: Routledge; 2013.
  53. Monacis L, de Palo V, Griffiths MD, Sinatra M. Social networking addiction, attachment style, and validation of the Italian version of the Bergen Social Media Addiction Scale. J Behav Addict 2017 Jun 1;6(2):178-186 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  54. Schimmenti A, Passanisi A, Gervasi AM, Manzella S, Famà FI. Insecure attachment attitudes in the onset of problematic Internet use among late adolescents. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev 2014 Oct;45(5):588-595. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  55. Goetz C. Securing home base: separation-individuation, attachment theory, and the 'virtual worlds' paradigm in video games. Psychoanal Study Child 2017 Mar;70(1):101-116. [CrossRef]
  56. Weisskirch RS, Delevi R. Its ovr b/n u n me: technology use, attachment styles, and gender roles in relationship dissolution. Cyberpsychol Behav Soc Netw 2012 Sep;15(9):486-490. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  57. Lama D. Stages of Meditation: Training the Mind for Wisdom. Stages of Meditation: Training the Mind for Wisdom. Random House; 2011.
  58. American PA. American Psychiatric Association Practice Guideline for the Treatment of Patients with Major Depressive Disorder.3rd ed. Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing; 2010.
  59. Segal ZV, Williams JM, Teasdale JD. Mindfulness-Based Cognitive Therapy for Depression. USA: The Guilford Press; 2012.
  60. Spears CA, Hedeker D, Li L, Wu C, Anderson NK, Houchins SC, et al. Mechanisms underlying mindfulness-based addiction treatment versus cognitive behavioral therapy and usual care for smoking cessation. J Consult Clin Psychol 2017 Nov;85(11):1029-1040. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  61. Shonin E, Van Gordon W, Griffiths MD. Ontological addiction: classification, etiology, and treatment. Mindfulness 2016 Jun;7(3):660-671. [CrossRef]
  62. Khanna S, Greeson JM. A narrative review of yoga and mindfulness as complementary therapies for addiction. Complement Ther Med 2013 Jun;21(3):244-252 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  63. Chiesa A, Serretti A. Mindfulness based cognitive therapy for psychiatric disorders: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychiatry Res 2011 May 30;187(3):441-453. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  64. Arslan G. Psychological maltreatment, forgiveness, mindfulness, and Internet addiction among young adults: a study of mediation effect. Comput Human Behav 2017 Jul;72:57-66. [CrossRef]
  65. Li W, Garland EL, O’Brien JE, Tronnier C, McGovern P, Anthony B, et al. Mindfulness-oriented recovery enhancement for video game addiction in emerging adults: preliminary findings from case reports. Int J Ment Health Addict 2017:1-18. [CrossRef]
  66. Shonin E, Van Gordon W, Griffiths MD. Mindfulness in psychology: a breath of fresh air? Psychologist 2015 Jan;28:28-31 [FREE Full text]
  67. Pepping CA, O’Donovan A, Davis PJ. The differential relationship between mindfulness and attachment in experienced and inexperienced meditators. Mindfulness 2014 Aug;5(4):392-399. [CrossRef]
  68. Walsh JJ, Balint M, Smolira SJ DR, Fredericksen LK, Madsen S. Predicting individual differences in mindfulness: the role of trait anxiety, attachment anxiety and attentional control. Pers Indiv Differ 2009;46:94-99. [CrossRef]
  69. Pepping CA, Davis PJ, O’Donovan A. Individual differences in attachment and dispositional mindfulness: the mediating role of emotion regulation. Pers Individ Dif 2013 Feb;54(3):453-456. [CrossRef]
  70. Pepping CA, O’Donovan A, Zimmer-Gembeck MJ, Hanisch M. Individual differences in attachment and eating pathology: the mediating role of mindfulness. Pers Individ Dif 2015;75:24-29. [CrossRef]
  71. Goodall K, Trejnowska A, Darling S. The relationship between dispositional mindfulness, attachment security and emotion regulation. Pers Individ Dif 2012;52(5):622-626. [CrossRef]
  72. Shaver PR, Lavy S, Saron CD, Mikulincer M. Social foundations of the capacity for mindfulness: an attachment perspective. Psychol Inq 2007;18(4):264-271. [CrossRef]
  73. Sümer N. Categorical and dimensional comparison of the adult attachment measures. Turkish J Psychol 2006;21:1-22.
  74. Brown KW, Ryan RM. The benefits of being present: mindfulness and its role in psychological well-being. J Pers Soc Psychol 2003 Apr;84(4):822-848. [Medline]
  75. Özyeşil Z, Arslan C, Kesici S, Deniz MN. Adaptation of the mindful attention awareness scale into Turkish. Educ Sci 2011;36:224-235.
  76. Yildirim C, Sumuer E, Adnan M, Yildirim S. A growing fear: prevalence of nomophobia among Turkish college students. Info Dev 2016 Aug;32(5):1322-1331. [CrossRef]
  77. Hair Jr JF, Tatham R, Anderson R, Black C. Multivariate Data Analysis. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson Prentice Hall; 2006.
  78. Kline RB. Principles and Practice of Structural Equation Modeling. New York, NY: The Guilford Press; 2005.
  79. Hoyle RH. In: Hoyle RH, editor. The Structural Equation Modeling Approach: Basic Concepts and Fundamental Issues. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications; 1995:1-15.
  80. Yao YW, Chen PR, Li CR, Hare TA, Li S, Zhang JT, et al. Combined reality therapy and mindfulness meditation decrease intertemporal decisional impulsivity in young adults with Internet gaming disorder. Comput Human Behav 2017 Mar;68:210-216 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef]
  81. Van Gordon W, Shonin E, Griffiths MD. Meditation awareness training for the treatment of sex addiction: a case study. J Behav Addict 2016 Jun;5(2):363-372 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  82. Garland EL, Froeliger B, Howard MO. Mindfulness training targets neurocognitive mechanisms of addiction at the attention-appraisal-emotion interface. Front Psychiatry 2014 Jan 10;4:173 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  83. Black DS. Mindfulness-based interventions: an antidote to suffering in the context of substance use, misuse, and addiction. Subst Use Misuse 2014 Apr;49(5):487-491. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  84. Peltz L, Black DS. The thinking mind as addiction: mindfulness as antidote. Subst Use Misuse 2014 Apr;49(5):605-607. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  85. Garland EL. Restructuring reward processing with mindfulness-oriented recovery enhancement: novel therapeutic mechanisms to remediate hedonic dysregulation in addiction, stress, and pain. Ann N Y Acad Sci 2016 Jun;1373(1):25-37 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  86. Durkee T, Kaess M, Carli V, Parzer P, Wasserman C, Floderus B, et al. Prevalence of pathological internet use among adolescents in Europe: demographic and social factors. Addiction 2012 Dec;107(12):2210-2222. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  87. Bem SL. Gender schema theory and its implications for child development: raising gender-aschematic children in a gender-schematic society. J Women Cult Soc 1983;8(4):598-616. [CrossRef]
  88. Katz D, Toner B. A systematic review of gender differences in the effectiveness of mindfulness-based treatments for substance use disorders. Mindfulness 2013;4(4):318-331. [CrossRef]


AGFI: Adjusted Goodness-of-Fit Index
AVE: average variance extracted
CFA: confirmatory factor analysis
CFI: Comparative Fit Index
CR: composite reliability
ECR: Experiences in Close Relationships
GFI: Goodness-of-Fit Index
IFI: Incremental Fit Index
MAAS: Mindful Attention Awareness Scale
NFI: Normed Fit Index
NMP-Q: Nomophobia Questionnaire
RMR: root mean square residual
RMSEA: root mean square error of approximation
SD: standard deviation
SE: standard error
SEM: structural equation modeling
TLI: Tucker-Lewis Index


Edited by G Eysenbach; submitted 25.08.17; peer-reviewed by R Sahin, Y Aydın Son, C Eichenberg; comments to author 12.10.17; revised version received 14.10.17; accepted 29.10.17; published 14.12.17

Copyright

©Ibrahim Arpaci, Mustafa Baloğlu, Hatice İrem Özteke Kozan, Şahin Kesici. Originally published in the Journal of Medical Internet Research (http://www.jmir.org), 14.12.2017.

This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work, first published in the Journal of Medical Internet Research, is properly cited. The complete bibliographic information, a link to the original publication on http://www.jmir.org/, as well as this copyright and license information must be included.