Published on in Vol 23, No 9 (2021): September

Preprints (earlier versions) of this paper are available at https://preprints.jmir.org/preprint/27634, first published .
Effects of Message Framing on Cancer Prevention and Detection Behaviors, Intentions, and Attitudes: Systematic Review and Meta-analysis

Effects of Message Framing on Cancer Prevention and Detection Behaviors, Intentions, and Attitudes: Systematic Review and Meta-analysis

Effects of Message Framing on Cancer Prevention and Detection Behaviors, Intentions, and Attitudes: Systematic Review and Meta-analysis

Review

1School of Information Management, Wuhan University, Wuhan, China

2School of Public Health, Fudan University, Shanghai, China

Corresponding Author:

Feicheng Ma, MSc

School of Information Management, Wuhan University

No 299 Bayi Road, Wuchang District

Wuhan

China

Phone: 86 86 13507119710

Email: fchma@whu.edu.cn


Background: With the increasing health care burden of cancer, public health organizations are increasingly emphasizing the importance of calling people to engage in long-term prevention and periodical detection. How to best deliver behavioral recommendations and health outcomes in messaging is an important issue.

Objective: This study aims to disaggregate the effects of gain-framed and loss-framed messages on cancer prevention and detection behaviors and intentions and attitudes, which has the potential to inform cancer control programs.

Methods: A search of three electronic databases (Web of Science, Scopus, and PubMed) was conducted for studies published between January 2000 and December 2020. After a good agreement achieved on a sample by two authors, the article selection (κ=0.8356), quality assessment (κ=0.8137), and data extraction (κ=0.9804) were mainly performed by one author. The standardized mean difference (attitude and intention) and the odds ratio (behaviors) were calculated to evaluate the effectiveness of message framing (gain-framed message and loss-framed message). Calculations were conducted, and figures were produced by Review Manager 5.3.

Results: The title and abstract of 168 unique citations were scanned, of which 53 were included for a full-text review. A total of 24 randomized controlled trials were included, predominantly examining message framing on cancer prevention and detection behavior change interventions. There were 9 studies that used attitude to predict message framing effect and 16 studies that used intention, whereas 6 studies used behavior to examine the message framing effect directly. The use of loss-framed messages improved cancer detection behavior (OR 0.76, 95% CI 0.64-0.90; P=.001), and the results from subgroup analysis indicated that the effect would be weak with time. No effect of framing was found when effectiveness was assessed by attitudes (prevention: SMD=0.02, 95% CI –0.13 to 0.17; P=.79; detection: SMD=–0.05, 95% CI –0.15 to 0.05; P=.32) or intentions (prevention: SMD=–0.05, 95% CI –0.19 to 0.09; P=.48; detection: SMD=0.02, 95% CI –0.26 to 0.29; P=.92) among studies encouraging cancer prevention and cancer detection.

Conclusions: Research has shown that it is almost impossible to change people's attitudes or intentions about cancer prevention and detection with a gain-framed or loss-framed message. However, loss-framed messages have achieved preliminary success in persuading people to adopt cancer detection behaviors. Future studies could improve the intervention design to achieve better intervention effectiveness.

J Med Internet Res 2021;23(9):e27634

doi:10.2196/27634

Keywords



Background

Cancer accounts for 1 in 6 deaths globally. The number of cancer cases worldwide may increase by 60%, especially in low-income and middle-income countries where the increase may be as high as 81%, as estimated by the latest World Cancer Report [1]. This trend will undoubtedly lead to socioeconomic pressure and a shortage of medical resources. The current clinical research on cancer medicine mainly focuses on finding the cause of cancer and preventing the spread of cancer cells in the early stage [2,3]. However, these findings can be helpful only if people are sufficiently aware of adopting healthy behavior (ie, prevention and detection). This is a unique form of a health crisis that requires intensive communicative efforts. Therefore, messages need to be carefully designed in cancer communication to achieve positive health outcomes.

Message framing is an effective technique to change health behavior [4-10]. Health messaging attempts to change people's attitudes, intentions, or behaviors toward a specific health topic by emphasizing the expected benefits of undergoing specific health behaviors (ie, gain-framed messaging) or the possible loss if specific health behaviors are not done (ie, loss-framed messaging), to persuade people to follow healthy guidelines. According to the theory of reasoned action, attitude (or intention) is an essential direct predictor of behavior [11-13]. Moreover, attitudes, intentions, and behaviors are the standard measures of the effectiveness of health messages [4,14].

Health behaviors include disease prevention and detection behavior [4,6]. The former aims to avoid illness or deterioration, while the latter aims to reflect the presence or absence of risk. Gain-framed messaging fulfills the promise of a safer and more certain disease prevention measure in terms of disease prevention, so it is more effective than loss-framed messaging [4,15]. O’Keefe and Jensen [16] also showed the advantage of the gain-framed messaging in disease prevention, but they indicate that it does not apply to skin cancer [17,18]. On the other hand, in terms of disease detection, it was thought that the loss-framed messaging showed a higher persuasive effect [4,15]. However, the meta-analysis results show no significant difference between gain and loss framing [6,16]. Thus, apart from demonstrating the contribution of loss-framed messaging to breast cancer detection [17], there has been no definitive conclusion regarding cancer and message framing.

Objectives

The concept of message framing is an essential strategy in health promotion, but research on how to optimally frame cancer prevention and detection messaging is scarce. Furthermore, while the effectiveness of message framing in cancer prevention and detection remains unclear, there is still an increasing number of related studies that combine message framing with other variables (ie, color) to verify the strengthening or weakening of the framing effect. Therefore, a thorough examination of the effectiveness of the message framing in the context of cancer is needed.

This review mainly aims to systematically summarize the characteristics of the relevant intervention studies and then pool the effect sizes from the relevant studies to quantify the effects of those interventions on attitude, intention, and behavior change. The findings of this review can provide recommendations for researchers and clinicians to design effective messages in cancer communication.


Search Strategies

This review was conducted and is reported according to the PRISMA (preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses) guidelines. As mentioned in the initial registration (registration ID CRD42021252658), a series of structured electronic searches were performed in three English databases, including Web of Science, Scopus, and PubMed, focusing on the effectiveness of message framing related to a cancer topic and dated from January 1, 2000, to December 31, 2020. The procedures guiding article inclusion are presented in the flow chart in Figure 1. Example search terms are as follows: goal fram*, loss fram*, message fram*, goal fram*, health message, health infor*AND attitude, intention, behavio*, behavio*intention, behavio* chang* AND cancer, screen*, prevention, detection, AND specific validated database filters for randomized controlled trials

All articles identified in the search strategy were exported into reference management software (version 2.48.0; Mendeley) for duplicate checking and further screening. The reference lists of eligible articles were further reviewed to identify other relevant studies. Relevant reviews that emerged from the search strategy were checked for any additional studies. Grey literature (ie, working papers, unpublished studies, conference proceedings or abstracts, and dissertations) was not considered eligible. The preliminary retrieval process was taken as the total literature volume combining the reviews independently completed by two authors. The authors selected a sample of eligible studies after achieving good agreement (κ=0.8356), with the remainder being selected by one author.

Figure 1. Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analysis flowchart.
View this figure

Study Inclusion and Exclusion Criteria

Studies were included if they met the following criteria: (1) participants (not cancer patient or survivors) of both genders and any age range exposed to either the loss-framed or the gain-framed cancer prevention or detection (not treatment) message in experimental research; (2) interventions that have been delivered via either emphasizing the expected benefits of taking specific health behaviors (gain-framed message) or the possible loss of not taking specific health behaviors (loss-framed message); (3) the gain-framed and loss-framed message must come in pairs as a comparison and related to a specific cancer topic, not cancer in general; (4) the effectiveness of cancer-related messages was measured in terms of change in attitude (by scale), intention (by scale), and behavior (by yes or no), and the analysis of primary data was reported, and sufficient quantitative data was provided for estimating the total effect size. In addition, only randomized controlled trials (RCTs) were included. Finally, published studies that repeatedly used the same data were excluded, leaving only one study that reported the most complete results.

Data Extraction

Data from a sample of eligible studies was extracted by two authors, and after they achieved good agreement (κ=0.9804), the remainder was extracted by one author. The main framework of the extraction criteria was drawn by the authors altogether. The following information was extracted: (1) basic study characteristics including the first author, publication date, country, and funding of the study; (2) participant characteristics including sample size, age, ethnic, and gender ratio (female); (3) intervention characteristics including cancer type, research setting (lab or not), message contents, message resources, message delivery channel, intervention duration, and underpinning theories; (4) outcome measures including measurement of the outcomes; and (5) main results including intervention completion ratio and converted effect size (standardized mean difference [SMD] and odds ratio [OR]).

Bias Assessments

The risk of bias for RCTs was independently assessed by two authors (κ=0.8137) using the Cochrane Collaboration tool for assessing bias [19]. Differences of opinion were discussed and agreed upon by the two authors.

Strategy for Data Synthesis and Meta-Analysis

All meta-analyses were performed with Review Manager 5.3 (version 5.3; The Cochrane Collaboration) [20]. This study performed meta-analyses on the three message effectiveness indicators (attitude, intention, and behavior). Among them, for the two continuous variables (attitude and intention), the SMD of the gain-frame (as the intervention group) and the loss-frame (as the control group) are calculated to represent an effect size. Moreover, two subgroups of cancer prevention and cancer detection were designed separately and analyzed under each indicator. For the two-category variable (behavior), the OR of the gain-frame (as the intervention group) and the loss-frame (as the control group) are calculated to represent an effect size. In addition, two subgroups of cancer detection within 6 months (<6 months) or more than 6 months (>6 months) after the experiment were designed for comparison and analysis of the impact of the timing of the framing effect on cancer detection behavior. All meta-analyses used random-effects models. I2 statistics are used to determine heterogeneity and measure the degree of inconsistency. I2 values are 25%, 50%, and 75%, corresponding to low, medium, and high levels of heterogeneity, respectively. For high heterogeneity, the source of heterogeneity was investigated by conducting a subgroup analysis to explore potential moderators and demonstrate why heterogeneity existed. We used the funnel plot to observe whether there is publication bias and used the Egger regression line to confirm possible publication bias further.


Study Characteristics

The literature search identified a total of 319 studies extracted by two authors. After deduplication, a total of 168 studies’ titles and abstracts were screened, with 53 remaining for full-text screening. Among those, 24 articles met the inclusion criteria, and 4 studies met the inclusion criteria but did not meet the exclusion criteria. The authors attempted to obtain the research data of 8 studies that met the first two inclusion criteria but failed to provide usable data by contacting their corresponding authors via email and received 2 replies [21,22]. Due to the oversize issue, the main content of each study is summarized in Multimedia Appendix 1. Of the 24 studies used in the final analysis, 13 were conducted by the United States [21,23-34], 2 by the United Kingdom [35,36], South Korea [22,37], and China [38,39], and 1 by Australia [40], Ireland [41], Italy [42], Singapore [43], and South Africa [44]. Among the studies used, 5 were funded by the government or institute grant [22,23,25,26,38], 2 by the institute and the center [24,29], 2 by the institute and universities [30,34], 2 by universities [28,37], and 1 by the health charity and behavioral insights project [35]. The remaining 12 studies did not report receiving any funding [21,27,31-33,36,39-44].

The total sample size was 11,637, ranging from n=85 (intervention group) to n=752 (control group). The participants’ average age ranged from 12 to 91 years. Because some cancers are associated with a particular sex, 2 studies notably recruited all-male participants [32,42], while another 10 studies recruited all-female participants [21,23,24,26,30,34,35,37,39,44]. Also, there was a range of different cancer topics targeted during the study interventions, including 9 studies for skin cancer [22,25,27,31,33,36,40,41,45], 2 for colorectal cancer [28,38], and 1 for lung cancer [29].

Most of the experiments were conducted online, through email [27-29,40,43,44], a phone call [23] or text message [35], and other methods like webpage URLs [31,37]. There are also a small number of experiments that were done in a laboratory setting using traditional printed materials [21,22,25,32,33,41,42,45]. Messages were delivered in a variety of ways, including text only [27,28,36,44], text plus images or graphics [26,29,38,40], and video [24,30,34,39,43]. The majority of studies were based on prospect theory [21,22,24,27,29,32,34,37,41,43-45], and some of them used theory of planned (or reasoned) behavior [28,31,38,39], self-affirmation theory [36], and the health belief model [30,42]. There are also some studies that did not use theories or models [23,25,35]. The majority of studies focused on attitude [28,31,32,37-40,42,43] and intention [21,22,25-29,33,36,37,39-43,45], whereas 6 studies used behavior to examine the message framing effect directly [23,24,30,34,35,44].

Bias and Heterogeneity Assessments

As shown in Figures 2 and 3, the risk of bias assessment indicated that the included articles were relatively high in quality. For the publication bias examination, the funnel plot was used for preliminary identification and the Egger regression line to confirm the possible publication bias further.

Figure 2. Risk of bias summary of the individual studies (k=24).
View this figure
Figure 3. Risk of bias graph.
View this figure

Intervention Effectiveness

In the following sections, the synthesized results regarding attitude-related, intention-related, and behavior-related outcomes are introduced individually. The main results are visualized in the forest plot.

Attitude-Related Outcomes

Attitudes are commonly defined as “a psychological tendency that is expressed by evaluating a particular entity with some degree of favor or disfavor.” In this study, an attitude refers to the tendency towards engaging in a particular behavior or object targeted by a study's intervention.

The attitude was used to indicate the persuasiveness of cancer-related messages in 9 studies. The relevant data involved a total of 3277 participants, including 1647 in the gain group and 1630 in the loss group. As seen in Figure 4, no significant difference was found between the two groups in either cancer prevention attitude (SMD=0.02, 95% CI –0.13 to 0.17; P=.79) or cancer detection attitude (SMD=–0.05, 95% CI –0.15 to 0.05; P=.32). There was medium heterogeneity (I2=68%; P<.001) across the trials.

The medium heterogeneity can be attributed to the study by Kim [43] and Shen [31]. Participants of both genders were involved in Kim [43], which focused on breast cancer, while the other studies all invited gender-specific participants who were more likely to be affected by the cancer of interest. In addition, Shen [31] invited students to participate in the experiment, and women accounted for 69% of participants. As undergraduates may be more sensitive to information about skin cancer because they are more focused on appearance rather than because they perceive health risks, excluding these 2 studies did decrease the heterogeneity (I2=0%, P=.11), but no significant difference still existed between the gain and loss groups in either cancer prevention intention (SMD=–0.05, 95% CI –0.17 to 0.06; P=.37) or cancer detection intention (SMD=–0.05, 95% CI –0.15 to 0.04; P=.26).

The funnel plot (Multimedia Appendix 2) demonstrates relative symmetry for the studies of cancer detection or prevention attitude, which suggests no publication bias existed and the above analysis was reliable.

Figure 4. Forest plots of cancer detection attitude and cancer prevention attitude.
View this figure
Intention-Related Outcomes

Intention refers to a state of wanting, planning, or expecting to act in a given way. It can be general (ie, intending to mammogram) or specific (i.e., intending to mammogram in 3 months). In this study, the difference between intentions and other related cognitions such as expectations or willingness was not distinguished.

The intention was used to indicate the persuasiveness of cancer-related messages in 16 studies. The relevant data involved a total of 5289 participants, including 2763 in the gain group and 2526 in the loss group. As shown in Figure 5, no significant difference was found between the 2 groups in either cancer prevention intention (SMD=–0.05, 95% CI –0.19 to 0.09; P=.48) or cancer detection intention (SMD=0.02, 95% CI –0.26 to 0.29; P=.92). There was large heterogeneity (I2=78%; P<.001) across the trials.

Because the large heterogeneity can also be attributed to the study by Kim [43], excluding this study did decrease the heterogeneity (I2=34%, P=.09). In addition, the heterogeneity is due to the gender characteristics of the participants, as mentioned above. However, there is still no significant difference between the gain and loss groups in either cancer prevention intention (SMD=–0.05, 95% CI –0.19 to 0.09; P=.48) or cancer detection intention (SMD=–0.08, 95% CI –0.17 to 0.01; P=.08).

The funnel plot (Multimedia Appendix 3) demonstrates relative symmetry for the studies of cancer detection or prevention intention except for Kim [43], which suggests that there existed no publication bias after excluding this study.

Figure 5. Forest plots of cancer detection intention and cancer prevention intention.
View this figure
Behavior-Related Outcomes

In most cases, behavioral measures are obtained by researchers using self-reported data from participants through follow-up surveys. However, it does not rule out the existence of special access to behavioral data (eg, a health insurance database record).

Behavior was used to indicate the persuasiveness of messages about cancer detection in 6 studies. The relevant data involved a total of 3071 participants, including 1543 in the gain group and 1528 in the loss group. It should be mentioned that cancer detection behavior was measured twice in Schneider et al [30] and Consedine et al [23]. According to Figure 6, loss-framed messages were significantly more likely than gain-framed ones to persuade people into engaging in both cervical cancer detection behavior (OR 0.79, 95% CI 0.64-0.98; P=.03) and breast cancer detection behavior (OR 0.69, 95% CI 0.50-0.96; P=.03), with no heterogeneity (I2=13%; P=.33).

There was often a time interval between cancer detection behavior and the experiment in which the participants read the framed messages. For the five measurements of cancer detection behavior that were performed within 6 months after the experiments, loss-framed messages were still significantly more persuasive than gain-framed ones (OR 0.76, 95% CI 0.61-0.95; P=.01), with no heterogeneity (I2=0%; P=.82). However, the intervention of loss framing became ineffective beyond 6 months (OR 0.73, 95% CI 0.49-1.11; P=.14), with medium heterogeneity (I2=59%; P=.09).

The funnel plot (Multimedia Appendix 4) demonstrates asymmetry for the studies of cancer detection behavior, from which one cannot tell whether publication bias existed. However, the Egger linear regression analysis suggests there was no publication bias (P=.09).

Figure 6. Forest plots of cervical cancer detection behavior and breast cancer detection behavior.
View this figure
Relationships Between Behavior and Attitude or Intention

Given that significant results were obtained for cervical or breast cancer detection behavior, 4 studies of detection attitude or intention focusing on either of these two cancers were extracted to examine the relationships between behavior and attitude or intention. Excluding Kim [43] as a source of heterogeneity, the relevant data involved a total of 1284 participants, including 641 in the gain group and 643 in the loss group. No significant difference was found between the two groups in cancer detection attitude or intention (SMD=–0.05, 95% CI –0.14 to 0.03; P=.20), with no heterogeneity (I2=0.0%; P=.76). That is, as far as cervical and breast cancers are concerned, cancer detection behavior was not necessarily preceded by cancer detection attitude or intention. The funnel plot demonstrates relative symmetry for the studies of cancer detection attitude or intention, which suggests no publication bias existed, and the above analysis was reliable.


Principal Findings

This study meta-analyzed 24 empirical studies on the persuasive effect of message framing in cancer prevention and detection. By subdividing the convincing effect into three dimensions, attitude, intention, and behavior, the results show that gain-framed and loss-framed messages have no significant differences in people's attitudes and intentions to change their cancer prevention or detection behavior. However, for cancer detection behavior, especially breast and cervical cancer detection, loss-framed messaging is more persuasive than gain-framed messaging, and this effect gradually weakens over time.

This meta-analysis did not identify any advantages of gain-framed messaging. Previous studies have proposed that gain-framed messaging is more persuasive than loss-framed in disease prevention, especially preventative behavior [6,16]. Since no relevant data on prevention behavior in the cancer-related literature are included in this meta-analysis, it cannot further verify whether the predecessors' benefits in disease prevention behavior can be equally applicable to the cancer topic. The results also negated the comparative advantage of loss framing in disease detection. There are several possible reasons for this contradictory conclusion. First, the previous meta-analysis did not distinguish between the three measurement indicators, so the results were affected by a large number of measurement indicators [16]. Second, the amount of cancer-related literature included in the previous meta-analysis is small, so the conclusions drawn do not apply to all cancer topics [6]. Therefore, it is necessary to verify whether these existing conclusions can be applied to all health topics based on previous studies. Moreover, because of different health topics, the focus also varies. Some emphasize daily prevention (such as using sunscreen), while some require regular detection (such as a colonoscopy).

This meta-analysis also supports some of the results of previous studies. Neither gain-framed nor loss-framed messaging can exert the framing effect of changing people's attitudes or intentions [6]. Our meta-analysis includes multiple cancer themes, and it also expands the positive impact of the loss framing found in breast cancer detection [46].

One interesting finding of this meta-analysis is that the message framing effect is time-limited. Others have questioned the duration of the framing effect in explaining the declining return rate [34]. On the other hand, this meta-analysis evaluates a larger scale of data and finds that the framing effect diminishes over time. Therefore, future research could further investigate how often health information stimulation can exert the framing effect and determine the effect of repeating the same framing.

Another notable result of this study is the disconnect between people's attitudes, intentions, and behavior. When reorganizing data related to attitudes and intention for breast and cervical cancer prevention and detection, these results also show that attitudes and intentions cannot effectively predict behavior, indicating that there may indeed be a disconnect between people's attitudes, intentions, and behavior. This conclusion has also been confirmed by previous studies [6,47]. The ultimate purpose of any framed message, but especially a health message, is to promote a specific behavior. In the research process, paying too much attention to the influence of other variables besides behavior may outweigh the gains. Further verification is worthwhile to measure whether subjective attitudes and intentions to target behaviors can replace or predict the subsequent behavior after reading the framed message. The exploration in this area cannot stop at testing latent variables such as attitude and intention, and it should return its focus to “behavior” itself [48].

Limitations

As with all meta-analyses, this study was limited by the included literature measurement indicators and provided data. For example, the measurement data of attitude and intention included in the literature far exceed behavior-related data. Cancer detection behavior data were obtained only due to the lack of actual behavioral data related to cancer prevention. Also, the data are limited to two types of cancer (breast cancer and cervical cancer). Its generalizability also needs to be further verified based on more relevant research. Due to the low email response rate (2 out of 8), this meta-analysis excluded some studies meeting the inclusion criteria.

Conclusions

This meta-analysis shows that gain framing and loss framing have no significant difference in attitudes and intentions about cancer prevention and detection. On the other hand, loss framing can promote breast cancer and cervical cancer detection better than gain framing. However, the effect of the loss framing gradually weakens over time. Therefore, when constructing a message to promote cancer detection, more considerations can be given to loss framing’s promised short-term effects. People can be screened for related cancers quickly by emphasizing the possible risk of not performing cancer detection.

Acknowledgments

AA acknowledges financial support from the National Natural Science Foundation of China (grant 71661167007).

Authors' Contributions

AA and FM conceptualized the study design. AA and SZ collected and analyzed the data. AA, SZ, and XA interpreted the results and wrote the manuscript. AA and SZ revised the manuscript. All authors have read and approved the final draft of the manuscript.

Conflicts of Interest

None declared.

Multimedia Appendix 1

Detailed records of eligible studies.

DOCX File , 25 KB

Multimedia Appendix 2

Funnel plot depicting the publication bias of attitude-related studies.

PNG File , 45 KB

Multimedia Appendix 3

Funnel plot depicting the publication bias of intention-related studies.

PNG File , 40 KB

Multimedia Appendix 4

Funnel plot depicting the publication bias of behavior-related studies.

PNG File , 37 KB

  1. Wild C, Weiderpass E, Bernard WS. World Cancer Report: Cancer research for Cancer Prevention, 2020. International Agency of Research on Cancer, World Health Organization.   URL: https://publications.iarc.fr/586 [accessed 2021-01-10]
  2. Anderson RL, Balasas T, Callaghan J, Coombes RC, Evans J, Hall JA, Cancer Research UKCancer Therapeutics CRC Australia Metastasis Working Group. A framework for the development of effective anti-metastatic agents. Nat Rev Clin Oncol 2019 Mar;16(3):185-204 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  3. Piberger AL, Bowry A, Kelly RDW, Walker AK, González-Acosta D, Bailey LJ, et al. PrimPol-dependent single-stranded gap formation mediates homologous recombination at bulky DNA adducts. Nat Commun 2020 Nov 17;11(1):5863 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  4. Rothman AJ, Salovey P. Shaping perceptions to motivate healthy behavior: the role of message framing. Psychol Bull 1997 Jan;121(1):3-19. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  5. Akl EA, Oxman AD, Herrin J, Vist GE, Terrenato I, Sperati F, et al. Framing of health information messages. Cochrane Database Syst Rev 2011 Dec 07(12):CD006777. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  6. Gallagher KM, Updegraff JA. Health message framing effects on attitudes, intentions, and behavior: a meta-analytic review. Ann Behav Med 2012 Feb;43(1):101-116. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  7. Graham AL, Fang Y, Moreno JL, Streiff SL, Villegas J, Muñoz RF, et al. Online advertising to reach and recruit Latino smokers to an internet cessation program: impact and costs. J Med Internet Res 2012 Aug 27;14(4):135-147 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  8. Jiang T, Wu X, Chen Y, Wang Y. The effects of message framing on online health headline selection of female users: A moderation of approach/avoidance motivation. Int J Med Inform 2021 Apr;148:104397. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  9. Niemiec R, Jones MS, Mertens A, Dillard C. The effectiveness of COVID-related message framing on public beliefs and behaviors related to plant-based diets. Appetite 2021 May 14;165:105293. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  10. Xu X, Yang M, Zhao YC, Zhu Q. Effects of message framing and evidence type on health information behavior: the case of promoting HPV vaccination. AJIM 2020 Dec 07;73(1):63-79. [CrossRef]
  11. Triandis HC. Values, attitudes, and interpersonal behavior. Nebr Symp Motiv 1980;27:195-259. [Medline]
  12. Vezich I, Katzman P, Ames D, Falk E, Lieberman M. Modulating the neural bases of persuasion: why/how, gain/loss, and users/non-users. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 2017 Feb 01;12(2):283-297 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  13. Matkovic J, Clemens KS, Faasse K, Geers AL. Handwashing Message Type Predicts Behavioral Intentions in the United States at the Beginning of the Global COVID-19 Pandemic. Front Public Health 2021;9:583491 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  14. Ballard AM, Davis A, Hoffner CA. The Impact of Health Narratives on Persuasion in African American Women: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Health Commun 2021 May;36(5):560-571. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  15. Rothman A, Bartels R, Wlaschin J, Salovey P. The strategic use of gain- and loss-framed messages to promote healthy behavior: How theory can inform practice. Journal of Communication 2006;56(SUPPL):S202-S220. [CrossRef]
  16. O'keefe DJ, Jensen JD. Chapter 1: The Advantages of Compliance or the Disadvantages of Noncompliance? A Meta-Analytic Review of the Relative Persuasive Effectiveness of Gain-Framed and Loss-Framed Messages. Communication Yearbook 2006 Jan;30(1):1-43. [CrossRef]
  17. O'Keefe DJ, Jensen JD. The relative persuasiveness of gain-framed and loss-framed messages for encouraging disease prevention behaviors: a meta-analytic review. J Health Commun 2007;12(7):623-644. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  18. O'Keefe DJ, Wu D. Gain-framed messages do not motivate sun protection: a meta-analytic review of randomized trials comparing gain-framed and loss-framed appeals for promoting skin cancer prevention. Int J Environ Res Public Health 2012 Jun;9(6):2121-2133 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  19. Higgins JPT, Altman DG, Gøtzsche PC, Jüni P, Moher D, Oxman AD, et al. The Cochrane Collaboration's tool for assessing risk of bias in randomised trials. BMJ 2011;343:d5928 [FREE Full text] [Medline]
  20. Sterne JAC, Egger M, Sutton AJ. Meta-analysis software. In: Egger M, Davey-Smith G, Altman DG, editors. Systematic reviews in health care: meta-analysis in context. London, United Kingdom: BMJ Publishing Group; 2001:336-346.
  21. Gerend MA, Shepherd JE. Using message framing to promote acceptance of the human papillomavirus vaccine. Health Psychol 2007 Nov;26(6):745-752. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  22. Hwang Y, Cho H, Sands L, Jeong S. Effects of gain- and loss-framed messages on the sun safety behavior of adolescents: the moderating role of risk perceptions. J Health Psychol 2012 Sep;17(6):929-940. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  23. Consedine NS, Horton D, Magai C, Kukafka R. Breast screening in response to gain, loss, and empowerment framed messages among diverse, low-income women. J Health Care Poor Underserved 2007 Aug;18(3):550-566. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  24. Gallagher KM, Updegraff JA, Rothman AJ, Sims L. Perceived susceptibility to breast cancer moderates the effect of gain- and loss-framed messages on use of screening mammography. Health Psychol 2011 Mar;30(2):145-152 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  25. Jasper JD, Woolf J, Christman SD. Responding to framed health messages: different strokes for different (handedness) folks. Psychol Health 2014;29(6):671-686. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  26. Keller P, Lipkus I, Rimer B. Affect, Framing, and Persuasion. Journal of Marketing Research 2018 Oct 10;40(1):54-64. [CrossRef]
  27. Lee MJ, Kang H. Designing Skin Cancer Prevention Messages: Should We Emphasize Gains or Losses? Message Framing, Risk Type, and Prior Experience. Am J Health Promot 2018 May;32(4):939-948. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  28. Lucas T, Hayman LW, Blessman JE, Asabigi K, Novak JM. Gain versus loss-framed messaging and colorectal cancer screening among African Americans: A preliminary examination of perceived racism and culturally targeted dual messaging. Br J Health Psychol 2016 May;21(2):249-267. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  29. Mays D, Niaura RS, Evans WD, Hammond D, Luta G, Tercyak KP. Cigarette packaging and health warnings: the impact of plain packaging and message framing on young smokers. Tob Control 2015 Mar;24(e1):e87-e92 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  30. Schneider TR, Salovey P, Apanovitch AM, Pizarro J, McCarthy D, Zullo J, et al. The effects of message framing and ethnic targeting on mammography use among low-income women. Health Psychol 2001 Jul;20(4):256-266. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  31. Shen L, Mercer Kollar L. Testing Moderators of Message Framing Effect. Communication Research 2013 Jul 01;42(5):626-648 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef]
  32. Umphrey L. The effects of message framing and message processing on testicular self‐examination attitudes and perceived susceptibility. Communication Research Reports 2003 Mar;20(2):97-105. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  33. Voss RP, Corser R, McCormick M, Jasper JD. Influencing health decision-making: A study of colour and message framing. Psychol Health 2018 Jul;33(7):941-954. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  34. Rivers SE, Salovey P, Pizarro DA, Pizarro J, Schneider TR. Message framing and pap test utilization among women attending a community health clinic. J Health Psychol 2005 Jan;10(1):65-77. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  35. Huf S, Kerrison RS, King D, Chadborn T, Richmond A, Cunningham D, et al. Behavioral economics informed message content in text message reminders to improve cervical screening participation: Two pragmatic randomized controlled trials. Prev Med 2020 Oct;139:106170. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  36. Thomas K, Hevey D, Pertl M, Ní CS, Craig A, Maher L. Appearance matters: the frame and focus of health messages influences beliefs about skin cancer. Br J Health Psychol 2011 May;16(Pt 2):418-429. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  37. Han K, Jo S. Does culture matter?: a cross-national investigation of women's responses to cancer prevention campaigns. Health Care Women Int 2012;33(1):75-94. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  38. Lin C, Yeh W. How Does Health-Related Advertising with a Regulatory Focus and Goal Framing Affect Attitudes toward Ads and Healthy Behavior Intentions? Int J Environ Res Public Health 2017 Dec 04;14(12):1507 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  39. Tu Y, Lin Y, Fan L, Tsai T, Wang H. Effects of Multimedia Framed Messages on Human Papillomavirus Prevention Among Adolescents. West J Nurs Res 2019 Jan;41(1):58-77. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  40. Shao W. Framing and Efficacy: The Effect of Regulatory Fit on Skin Cancer Prevention and Detection. Journal of Nonprofit and Public Sector Marketing 2012 Jul;24(3):161-180. [CrossRef]
  41. Hevey D, Dolan M. Approach/avoidance motivation, message framing and skin cancer prevention: a test of the congruency hypothesis. J Health Psychol 2014 Aug;19(8):1003-1012. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  42. Cherubini P, Rumiati R, Rossi D, Nigro F, Calabrò A. Improving Attitudes Toward Prostate Examinations by Loss‐Framed Appeals. Journal of Applied Social Psychology 2006;35(4):732-742. [CrossRef]
  43. Kim HJ. The impacts of vicarious illness experience on response to gain- versus loss-framed breast cancer screening (BCS) messages. Health Commun 2014;29(9):854-865. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  44. Adonis L, Paramanund J, Basu D, Luiz J. Framing preventive care messaging and cervical cancer screening in a health-insured population in South Africa: Implications for population-based communication? J Health Psychol 2017 Sep;22(11):1365-1375. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  45. Hoffner C, Ye J. Young adults' responses to news about sunscreen and skin cancer: the role of framing and social comparison. Health Commun 2009 Apr;24(3):189-198. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  46. O'Keefe D, Jensen J. The Relative Persuasiveness of Gain-framed and Loss-framed Messages for Encouraging Disease Detection Behaviors: A Meta analytic Review. Journal of Communication (2) 2009:296-316. [CrossRef]
  47. Webb TL, Sheeran P. Does changing behavioral intentions engender behavior change? A meta-analysis of the experimental evidence. Psychol Bull 2006 Mar;132(2):249-268. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  48. Baumeister RF, Vohs KD, Funder DC. Psychology as the Science of Self-Reports and Finger Movements: Whatever Happened to Actual Behavior? Perspect Psychol Sci 2007 Dec;2(4):396-403. [CrossRef] [Medline]


OR: odds ratio
PRISMA: preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses
RCT: randomized controlled trial
SMD: standardized mean difference


Edited by R Kukafka; submitted 01.02.21; peer-reviewed by J Makin, X He; comments to author 16.04.21; revised version received 08.06.21; accepted 27.07.21; published 16.09.21

Copyright

©Abidan Ainiwaer, Shuai Zhang, Xiayiabasi Ainiwaer, Feicheng Ma. Originally published in the Journal of Medical Internet Research (https://www.jmir.org), 16.09.2021.

This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work, first published in the Journal of Medical Internet Research, is properly cited. The complete bibliographic information, a link to the original publication on https://www.jmir.org/, as well as this copyright and license information must be included.