Original Paper
Abstract
Background: The COVID-19 pandemic poses a major challenge to people’s everyday lives. In the context of hospitalization, the pandemic is expected to have a strong influence on affective reactions and preventive behaviors. Research is needed to develop evidence-driven strategies for coping with the challenges of the pandemic. Therefore, this survey study investigates the effects that personality traits, risk-taking behaviors, and anxiety have on medical service–related affective reactions and anticipated behaviors during the COVID-19 pandemic.
Objective: The aim of this study was to identify key factors that are associated with individuals’ concerns about hygiene in hospitals and the postponement of surgeries.
Methods: We conducted a cross-sectional, web-based survey of 929 residents in Germany (women: 792/929, 85.3%; age: mean 35.2 years, SD 12.9 years). Hypotheses were tested by conducting a saturated path analysis.
Results: We found that anxiety had a direct effect on people’s concerns about safety (β=−.12, 95% CI −.20 to −.05) and hygiene in hospitals (β=.16, 95% CI .08 to .23). Risk-taking behaviors and personality traits were not associated with concerns about safety and hygiene in hospitals or anticipated behaviors.
Conclusions: Our findings suggest that distinct interventions and information campaigns are not necessary for individuals with different personality traits or different levels of risk-taking behavior. However, we recommend that health care workers should carefully address anxiety when interacting with patients.
doi:10.2196/24804
Keywords
Introduction
In Germany, the first COVID-19 case was confirmed at the end of January 2020, and COVID-19 incidence rates rose in the following 3 months. In response, the Robert Koch Institute (ie, the German federal government agency and research institute responsible for disease control and prevention) and the Federal Centre for Health Education made the following recommendations to slow the interpersonal transmission of SARS-CoV-2: limit social contact, refrain from traveling unless absolutely necessary, work from home wherever possible, encourage the use of medical masks and gloves, and strengthen hand hygiene practices [
]. At the same time, the European Center for Disease Prevention and Control published a checklist to prepare hospitals for the reception and care of patients with COVID-19. This checklist included items that were related to hand hygiene, personal protective equipment, and the postponement of operations that were unrelated to COVID-19 [ ]. However, the implementation of these regulations, particularly those regarding the use of personal protective equipment during the initial weeks of the pandemic in Germany, was hindered by a lack of adequate medical masks and clothing [ ]. Considering the fact that SARS-CoV-2 infection can result in severe illness and death, especially in people aged >65 years and those with defined risk factors (eg, high blood pressure, diabetes, chronic respiratory diseases, and cancer) [ ], a lack of personal protective equipment in hospitals and inadequate medical practices can result in affective reactions (eg, worries and concerns) and anticipated behaviors (eg, the denial of important operations) among the general population [ , ].An example of an affective reaction resulting from a concern about an impending or anticipated threat is worrying about the lack of personal protective equipment in hospitals. Various factors, such as sociodemographic characteristics and personal values, can be used to predict affective reactions [
, ]. Further, affective reactions like concern or worry positively relate to anxiety [ ] and negatively relate to risk-taking behaviors [ ]. In addition, personality traits (eg, neuroticism) are linked with affective reactions [ ]. During the COVID-19 pandemic, it is necessary to investigate the possible antecedents of affective reactions that relate to hospital equipment and medical practices. Such information is necessary for training health care workers to develop psychological skills for helping patients who experience worry, anxiety, and other emotional problems. It is also necessary to investigate how the COVID-19 pandemic affects people’s reactions when they or a person close to them needs to be hospitalized to undergo surgery for treating an illness [ , ]. Studies have shown that the COVID-19 pandemic poses a considerable challenge to routine medical services. For example, a study reported that patients prefer to postpone their operations until after the pandemic has completely passed due to the uncertain environment [ ]. However, none of the studies that have been conducted during the pandemic have investigated psychological concepts that might influence individuals’ concerns about hospital hygiene and the postponement of surgeries. Studies on treatment-related decisions have suggested that personality traits, risk-taking behaviors, and anxiety are important factors that affect people’s decisions to avoid visiting a hospital or doctor [ , ].Based on previous pandemics, it is known that segmenting the population into subgroups (ie, sociodemographic subgroups) is important for designing and delivering messages about health risks and health protection measures [
, ]. However, even though this might be a useful and effective method, these subgroups do not account for several important psychological factors, such as personality traits or anxiety. These factors might be crucial antecedents of affective reactions to public health messages. They might also influence people’s health-related decisions. Specifically, these factors may directly affect anticipated behaviors that relate to people’s decisions to postpone a nonurgent surgery [ , ]. Therefore, this survey study aims to identify the key factors that are associated with hospitalization-related and medical service–related affective reactions and anticipated behaviors during the COVID-19 pandemic.We hypothesized that individuals with low levels of openness, high levels of conscientiousness, low levels of extraversion, low levels of agreeableness, high levels of neuroticism, low levels of risk-taking behavior, and high levels of anxiety would experience high levels of negative affective reactions and exhibit high levels of anticipated preventive behaviors in response to hospitalization and medical service provision.
Methods
Survey Summary
This cross-sectional, web-based survey study took place between March 19 and April 17, 2020. To ensure that our survey was highly visible to potential respondents, it was distributed via social media, email, direct communication methods, and advertisements in various digital communication channels. The recruitment of participants mainly took place at the Department of Psychology of Witten/Herdecke University. All participants were residents of Germany who were aged ≥16 years. All procedures in this study were performed in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional review board of the Department of Psychology and Psychotherapy of Witten/Herdecke University and those of the American Psychology Association [
, ]. A letter of approval can be obtained from the first author.Measures
Summary of Survey Instruments
Prior to the survey, we screened potentially eligible test instruments and scales to assess their suitability for answering the hypotheses. We selected validated scales (ie, whenever possible) for measuring the different survey constructs. We also developed new scales to measure the COVID-19–specific aspects of the survey, as no validated instruments were available at the time of the survey. The development of survey items was based on existing scales from other behavioral domains.
The following survey items, which were answered by using a visual analog scale that ranged from 0 (ie, not at all) to 100 (ie, absolutely), served as dependent variables: affective reactions and anticipated behaviors.
Affective Reactions
Affective reactions [
] were measured with two items for assessing concerns about hospital safety, hospital hygiene, and medical practices during the COVID-19 pandemic. After providing a short introduction to place the questions in the context of the COVID-19 pandemic, the following questions were asked: (1) “recently there have been supply bottlenecks of mouthguards, disinfectants or similar for hospitals and medical practices. Do you feel safe in places like this”; and (2) “how big is your concern that due to supply bottlenecks a proper hygiene cannot be ensured in hospitals or medical practices?”Anticipated Behaviors
Anticipated behaviors were measured with two items for assessing people’s decisions to postpone their own surgery or advise a person close to them against surgery during the pandemic. These items were in line with previous studies [
, ]. After providing a short introduction to place the questions in the context of the COVID-19 pandemic, the following questions were asked: (1) “assuming you were about to have a non-urgent surgery - how likely would you be to postpone this surgery”; and (2) “suppose a person very close to you was about to have a non-urgent surgery, how likely is it that you would advise against having the surgery?”The following survey items served as independent variables: personality, risk-taking behaviors, and anxiety.
Personality
People’s personalities were measured with the Big Five Inventory (BFI)-10, which is the short version of the BFI-44 [
]. The BFI assesses the following five personality traits: openness to experience, conscientiousness, extraversion, agreeableness, and neuroticism (OCEAN). Openness to experience refers to whether people are inventive/curious or consistent/cautious. Conscientiousness refers to whether people are efficient/organized or easygoing/careless. Extraversion refers to whether people are outgoing/energetic or solitary/reserved. Agreeableness refers to whether people are friendly/compassionate or challenging/detached. Neuroticism refers to whether people are sensitive/nervous or secure/confident. Our psychometric analyses indicated that BFI-10 scores sufficiently correlated with BFI-44 scores. Based on the average correlation value (r=0.83), 70% of the variance in BFI-44 scores could be explained. After 6-8 weeks, the BFI-10 had an average retest reliability value of 0.75.Risk-Taking Behaviors
Risk-taking behaviors were assessed with the readiness to take risk/search for competition scale of the Hamburger Personality Inventory (HPI), which includes 14 items that are evaluated with a 4-point Likert scale (eg, “Ultimately, I am also unstoppable by massive threats”). HPI item scores are added to calculate a risk-taking score [
]. With a Cronbach α value of .85, the HPI has high content and construct validity. The HPI has a test-retest reliability value of 0.86 after 18 months. Additionally, HPI scores positively correlate with autonomy orientations (r=0.48), revolutionary tendencies (r=0.53), conflict skills (r=0.53), and competitive attitudes (r=0.60). These scores also negatively correlate with harm avoidance tendencies (r=−0.78).Anxiety
Anxiety was measured with the German version of the Spielberger State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI), which is one of the most commonly used standard tools for measuring anxiety. In research, STAI scores also function as an indicator of distress. The state anxiety portion of the STAI consists of 20 items that are evaluated on a 4-point Likert scale (eg, “I feel worried”). All item scores are added to calculate a state anxiety score [
]. Higher STAI scores indicate greater anxiety/distress. The STAI has Cronbach α values that range between .90 and .94, which means that it has high content and construct validity. According to the original publication [ ], the test-retest reliability coefficients of the STAI range between 0.65 to 0.75 (ie, within 2 months of completing the STAI). These coefficients remained stable in our psychometric analyses.To assess whether people’s risk of contracting COVID-19 and information-seeking behaviors (ie, those related to COVID-19) had an impact on their worries and anticipated behaviors, the following constructs were included in our analysis as covariates: risk profile and information-seeking behaviors.
Risk Profile
Risk profiles were adapted in accordance with previous studies [
, ]. Our survey included seven dichotomous items (ie, yes=1; no=0) that asked about risk factors for contracting COVID-19 (ie, age of >60 years, chronic lung disease, autoimmune disease, diabetes, kidney or liver diseases, cancer, immune deficiency, and the intake of immunosuppressive remedies). The sum of the item scores was used as a risk profile.Information-Seeking Behaviors
Information-seeking behaviors were adapted in accordance with a previous study [
]. The behaviors we analyzed were in line with another study [ ]. Our survey included six dichotomous items (ie, yes=1; no=0) that asked about the sources that people used to obtain information on COVID-19 (ie, television, internet blogs, social media, the website of the German federal government agency that is responsible for disease control and prevention, newspapers, and tabloid press articles). The sum of the item scores was used as an indicator of information-seeking behavior intensity. In addition, age, gender, and educational level (ie, a dichotomous variable that accounted for primary and secondary education) were introduced in the path model as covariates that needed to be controlled.Statistical Analysis Strategy
Participants who fully completed the questionnaires were included in the statistical analysis. Descriptive statistical analyses were performed to describe the sample’s characteristics in terms of the variables that were included in this study. In addition, bivariate correlation values were computed to examine associations among the variables. A saturated path model [
] with manifest variables was used to test whether OCEAN personality traits, anxiety, and risk-taking behaviors were related to worries about hospital safety and hygiene, worries about medical practices, and anticipated behaviors toward nonurgent surgeries ( ). To assess whether people’s risk of contracting COVID-19 and information-seeking behaviors (ie, those related to COVID-19) had an impact on their worries and anticipated behaviors, these variables were included in the analysis.Age, gender, and educational level (ie, a dichotomous variable that accounted for primary and secondary education) were introduced in the model as covariates that needed to be controlled. All variables in the model were allowed to covary. Standardized regression coefficients (ie, βi) for the path model (ie, the model for predicting affective reactions) and anticipated behaviors were calculated with the decomposition equation of correlations (ie, ri), which is used to determine the direct and indirect effects that predictor variables (ie, Xi) have on dependent variables (ie, Yj). In addition, the 95% CIs were calculated based on the 2.5 and 97.5 percentiles of the estimated SEs from bootstrapping [
]. As our study had four main dependent variables, Bonferroni correction was performed to lower the Cronbach α level for interpreting the results (ie, from .05 to .0125). To evaluate model fitness, the Chi-square test was used. According to Bollen and Long [ ], Chi-square values should not be 2-5 times larger than the degrees of freedom. In addition, comparative fit index, Tucker-Lewis index, root mean square error of approximation, and standardized root mean square residual values were calculated as fit indices. Descriptive statistical analyses were conducted with SPSS, version 26 (IBM Corporation). The path analysis was conducted with Mplus, version 8.1 (Muthén & Muthén) [ ].Results
Descriptive Characteristics
Of the 1059 participants who took part in our survey, 929 (87.7%) had complete data sets. Thus, these 929 participants were included in the analyses. As indicated in
, most of the participants (792/929, 85.3%) were female. The mean age of participants was 35.3 years (SD 12.9 years). Of the 929 participants, 890 (95.8%) stated that they were not infected with SARS-CoV-2, and only 7 (0.8%) stated that they were infected with SARS-CoV-2 (ie, at the time of the survey or before the survey). With respect to risk profiles, 683 (73.5%) participants reported that they did not exhibit any of the risk factors for contracting COVID-19, while 112 (12.1%) stated that they had a chronic lung disease. Almost all participants (884/929, 95.2%) subjectively felt restricted due to COVID-19–related regulations and measures. Details on participants’ sociodemographic characteristics are provided in .Descriptive statistics and correlations among the variables in the path model are reported in
and . Worries about proper hospital hygiene and medical practices positively correlated with neuroticism (r=0.12) and anxiety (r=0.21). Further, all four dependent variables intercorrelated with each other. For example, worries about hygiene and worries about safety significantly correlated with each other (r=−0.40; P=.001).Sociodemographic variables | Value | ||
Age (years), mean (SD) | 35.3 (12.9) | ||
Age (years), median (range) | 32 (16-82) | ||
Sex, n (%) | |||
Male | 137 (14.7) | ||
Female | 792 (85.3) | ||
Educational level, n (%) | |||
No school degree | 3 (0.3) | ||
Secondary school | 6 (1.7) | ||
Secondary modern education | 108 (11.6) | ||
Vocational baccalaureate | 75 (8.1) | ||
General baccalaureate | 223 (24) | ||
Applied science university diploma | 116 (12.5) | ||
Bachelor’s degree | 181 (19.5) | ||
Master’s degree | 172 (18.5) | ||
Doctorate degree or higher | 34 (3.7) | ||
COVID-19 status, n (%) | |||
Not infected | 890 (95.8) | ||
I was under suspicion | 17 (1.8) | ||
I am under suspicion | 15 (1.6) | ||
I was infected | 5 (0.5) | ||
I am infected | 2 (0.2) | ||
Risk profile, n (%) | |||
No risk factors | 683 (73.5) | ||
Aged >60 years | 50 (5.4) | ||
Chronic lung disease | 112 (12.1) | ||
Autoimmune disease | 66 (7.1) | ||
Diabetes | 31 (3.3) | ||
Cancer | 15 (1.6) | ||
Immunodeficiency | 56 (6) | ||
Intake of immunosuppressants | 43 (4.6) | ||
Information source, n (%) | |||
Friends and family | 369 (39.7) | ||
Television | 553 (59.5) | ||
Internet in general | 401 (43.2) | ||
Social media | 402 (43.3) | ||
Dedicated websites | 758 (81.6) | ||
Newspapers | 495 (53.3) | ||
Tabloid press articles | 29 (3.1) | ||
Feeling restricted due to COVID-19–related regulations and measures, n (%) | |||
Yes | 884 (95.2) | ||
No | 45 (4.8) |
Variable | Conscientiousness | Extraversion | Agreeableness | Neuroticism | RTBa | Anxiety | Risk profile | Information profile | Feeling secureb | Hygieneb | Own surgeryc | Surgery of a close personc | |||||||||||||
Openness | |||||||||||||||||||||||||
r | 0.03 | 0.03 | 0.03 | −0.01 | 0.12e | 0.01 | 0.02 | −0.07e | 0.01 | 0.03 | 0.01 | <−0.01 | |||||||||||||
P value | .39 | .10 | .35 | .77 | <.001 | 0.7 | .57 | 0.05 | .78 | .38 | .73 | .91 | |||||||||||||
Conscientiousness | |||||||||||||||||||||||||
r | —d | 0.12e | 0.08e | −0.15e | 0.12e | −0.12e | 0.03 | −0.08e | 0.02 | −0.04 | 0.02 | 0.06 | |||||||||||||
P value | — | <.001 | .02 | <.001 | <.001 | <.001 | .38 | .02 | .59 | .25 | .51 | .09 | |||||||||||||
Extraversion | |||||||||||||||||||||||||
r | — | — | 0.13e | −0.30e | 0.22e | −0.22e | −0.01 | <0.01 | 0.05 | −0.11e | −0.01 | −0.02 | |||||||||||||
P value | — | — | <.001 | <.001 | <.001 | <.001 | .75 | .90 | .13 | <.001 | .88 | .57 | |||||||||||||
Agreeableness | |||||||||||||||||||||||||
r | — | — | — | −0.12e | −0.04 | −0.19e | −0.03 | 0.01 | 0.08e | -0.09e | −0.02 | <−0.01 | |||||||||||||
P value | — | — | — | <.001 | .23 | <.001 | .41 | .84 | .02 | .007 | .47 | .95 | |||||||||||||
Neuroticism | |||||||||||||||||||||||||
r | — | — | — | — | −0.32e | 0.48e | 0.03 | 0.03 | −0.05 | 0.13e | −0.01 | 0.01 | |||||||||||||
P value | — | — | — | — | <.001 | <.001 | .36 | .43 | .11 | <.001 | .69 | .79 | |||||||||||||
RTB | |||||||||||||||||||||||||
r | — | — | — | — | — | −0.20e | 0.04 | −0.04 | 0.04 | −0.05 | −0.09e | −0.06 | |||||||||||||
P value | — | — | — | — | — | <.001 | .24 | .23 | .19 | .14 | .007 | .07 | |||||||||||||
Anxiety | |||||||||||||||||||||||||
r | — | — | — | — | — | — | 0.09e | 0.07e | −0.14e | 0.21e | 0.08e | 0.06 | |||||||||||||
P value | — | — | — | — | — | — | .005 | .04 | <.001 | <.001 | .02 | .05 | |||||||||||||
Risk profile | |||||||||||||||||||||||||
r | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | 0.06 | −0.07e | 0.12e | 0.02 | 0.07e | |||||||||||||
P value | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | .08 | .03 | <.001 | .47 | .04 | |||||||||||||
Information profile | |||||||||||||||||||||||||
r | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | <−0.01 | 0.04 | −0.01 | 0.05 | |||||||||||||
P value | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | .98 | .25 | .77 | .16 | |||||||||||||
Feeling secureb | |||||||||||||||||||||||||
r | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | −0.40e | −0.13e | −0.13e | |||||||||||||
P value | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | <.001 | <.001 | <.001 | |||||||||||||
Hygieneb | |||||||||||||||||||||||||
r | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | 0.18e | 0.18e | |||||||||||||
P value | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | <.001 | <.001 | |||||||||||||
Own surgeryc | |||||||||||||||||||||||||
r | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | 0.70e | |||||||||||||
P value | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | <.001 |
aRTB: risk-taking behavior.
bRefers to a worry.
cRefers to an anticipated behavior category.
dNot applicable.
eSignificant at a level of P<.05.
Variable | Value, mean (SD) |
Openness | 7.60 (2.02) |
Conscientiousness | 7.15 (1.65) |
Extraversion | 6.66 (1.97) |
Agreeableness | 6.20 (1.58) |
Neuroticism | 6.26 (2.04) |
Risk-taking behavior | 31.26 (7.13) |
Anxiety | 43.91 (12.23) |
Risk profile | 0.40 (0.80) |
Information profile | 3.24 (1.36) |
Worries about feeling secure | 48.32 (28.23) |
Worries about hygiene | 57.33 (30.35) |
Anticipated behavior relating to own surgery | 80.46 (28.45) |
Anticipated behavior relating to the surgery of a close person | 77.32 (28.93) |
Results From the Path Model
The path model predicted the associations between independent variables (ie, personality, risk-taking behaviors, and anxiety) and dependent variables (ie, feelings about security, worries about hospital hygiene and medical practices, and anticipated behaviors that relate to people’s decisions to postpone their own surgery or advise a person close to them against surgery).
presents the parameter estimates of the model (ie, standardized solutions).The following model-data fit indices were obtained: Chi-square value (χ254=942.94; N=929; P<.001), comparative fit index (1.00), Tucker-Lewis Index (1.00), root mean square error of approximation (<.01), and standardized root mean square residual (<.01). These values indicated a moderate model fitness.
provides the standardized regression coefficients of the path model, which was used to predict affective reactions and anticipated behaviors.Path predictors | Affective reactions | Anticipated behaviors | ||
Feeling secure, β (95% CI) | Concerns about hygiene, β (95% CI) | Own surgery, β (95% CI) | Surgery of a close person, β (95% CI) | |
Openness | <.01 (−.07 to .08) | .02 (−.05 to .09) | .02 (−.04 to .09) | −.02 (−.09 to .05) |
Conscientiousness | .01 (−.05 to .08) | −.03a (−.09 to .04) | .01 (−.06 to .08) | .03 (−.03 to .10) |
Extraversion | .03 (−.04 to .10) | −.07a (−.14 to <.01) | .01 (−.06 to .08) | −.02 (−.09 to .05) |
Agreeableness | .05 (−.02 to .12) | −.05a (−.11 to .02) | −.03 (−.10 to .05) | <.01 (−.06 to .07) |
Neuroticism | .05 (−.03 to .13) | .01 (−.07 to .09) | −.08b (−.16 to −.01) | −.02 (−.10 to .06) |
Risk-taking behavior | .01 (−.07 to .08) | <.01 (−.07 to .08) | −.09b (−.16 to −.01) | −.04 (−.12 to .03) |
Anxiety | −.12c (−.20 to −.05) | .16d (.08 to .23) | .08b (.01 to .16) | .05 (−.03 to .13) |
Risk profile | −.06 (−.14 to .01) | .08b (.02 to .14) | <.01 (−.07 to .08) | .01 (−.05 to .08) |
Information-seeking behavior | −.01 (−.09 to .07) | −.01 (−.07 to .05) | .01 (−.14 to .15) | .03 (−.05 to .10) |
Gendere | .09b (.02 to .17) | −.06a (−.12 to .01) | −.08a (−.16 to .01) | −.08b (−.15 to −.01) |
Age | −.05 (−.13 to .03) | .10c (.03 to .17) | .09b (.01 to .16) | .18d (.11 to .25) |
Educationf | .03 (−.05 to .11) | −.08b (−.16 to <−.01) | −.06 (−.16 to .04) | −.11c (−.19 to −.03) |
aSignificant at a level of P<.10.
bSignificant at a level of P<.05.
cSignificant at a level of P<.001.
dSignificant at a level of P<.001.
eIn the path model, women were given a value of 1 and men were given a value of 2.
fIn the path model, secondary education was given a value of 1 and tertiary education was given a value of 2.
As outlined in
, the feeling of security with regard to hospitals and medical practices was significantly negatively related to anxiety (β=−.12; P=.001), which is in line with our hypothesis. Further, affective reactions to hospital hygiene and medical practices resulting from a bottleneck of appropriate personal protective equipment for health care workers were significantly positively related to anxiety (β=.16; P<.001) and nonsignificantly negatively related to extraversion (β=−.07; P=.054). Although anticipated behaviors that relate to advising a close person against surgery did not correlate with any of our hypothesized variables, anticipated behaviors that relate to one’s own surgery were negatively associated with neuroticism (β=−.08; P=.04) and risk-taking behaviors (β=−.09; P=.03). Such anticipated behaviors were also positively associated with anxiety (β=.08; P=.03). All of these associations however were not statistically significant after the Bonferroni correction. No other associations between the independent and dependent variables were found. However, women and older participants reported that they experienced higher levels of negative affective reactions and anticipated behaviors compared to men and younger participants, respectively.Discussion
Principal Findings
To the best of our knowledge, our study is the first to investigate predictors of affective reactions that relate to hospital safety, hospital hygiene, and medical practices during the COVID-19 pandemic. We are also the first to investigate anticipated behaviors that relate to people’s decisions to postpone their surgery or advise a person close to them against surgery during the pandemic. Our findings are in line with those of a German-Austrian survey [
], which found that anxiety was positively related to security actions. Our results suggest that state anxiety is the most influential factor of anticipated health-related behaviors and concerns about safety or hygiene. Apart from state anxiety, none of the other hypothesized predictors (eg, risk-taking behaviors) or personality factors (eg, agreeableness or openness) had any significant association with affective reactions or anticipated behaviors. This is contradictory to the recent findings of Martin [ ], who found that agreeableness was related to the perceived severity of the COVID-19 pandemic, and openness was related to low levels of anxiety with regard to contracting COVID-19. Although previous studies have suggested that individuals with high levels of neuroticism exhibit pronounced negative reactions to stressful events [ ], our findings show that neuroticism was not associated with anticipated behaviors during the COVID-19 pandemic. In our study, we found that people with high levels of neuroticism were less likely to postpone their own surgery. This finding is comparable to that of an early US survey, which found that neuroticism was associated with high levels of concern [ ]. The similarities in these results could be explained by the age of our participants. It is possible that our relatively young participants were not able to accurately imagine a scenario in which they are hospitalized. This might have influenced participants’ responses to our survey. With regard to the relationship between health policy formation and public responses [ ], the most important finding of our study was that anxiety was related to both the affective reactions and anticipated behaviors of the participants. Allgleton and Kippax [ ], who conducted an analysis on Australian HIV/AIDS policies, argued that suppressed anxiety can be used as a depressive position for eliciting a desired response in the general public [ ]. Other studies [ - ] have also found that anxiety is an important predictive factor of taking preventive measures and exhibiting compliant behavioral responses during the 2009 influenza pandemic. Our findings support these empirical results. Health authorities should be aware that anxiety may not only affect individuals’ behaviors but also the behaviors of organizations and systems (eg, splitting and blaming) [ ]. In addition, anxiety resulting from the COVID-19 pandemic might also encourage individuals to consult a physician later than necessary (ie, to present their complaints). This has already occurred [ ]. Such behavior may result in harms to health, the development of depression [ ], or the chronification of disease.Limitations and Implications for Future Research
Aside from the strengths of our study (eg, its large sample size), several limitations also need to be mentioned. First, due to the dynamic nature of the pandemic, we decided to use a random sample. However, due to our survey dissemination methods, our sample may not be representative of the German population. The generalizability of our results is open to empirical debate, as our sample mostly consisted of middle-aged and well-educated women. Research has shown that compared to men, women are more likely to actively seek health-related information and pay more attention to potential worldwide pandemics [
]. Second, our sample mainly consisted of middle-aged individuals. Therefore, it is reasonable to assume that older people are more likely to postpone surgeries and operations due to the COVID-19 pandemic, as they are more susceptible to the disease than middle-aged people. Older people also have stronger health care needs than middle-aged people [ ]. Further research on the COVID-19–related concerns of older individuals is needed. Third, data collection took place during the beginning of the pandemic in Germany. Therefore, it remains unclear whether individuals would have the same affective reactions and anticipated behaviors later into the pandemic. Furthermore, the affective reactions and anticipated behaviors of people from urban areas should be distinguished from those of people from rural areas, as COVID-19 spreads at different rates in different geographical areas [ ]. However, during the first phase of the pandemic in Germany, no considerable differences were found in infection and death rates [ ]. This finding is also supported by the results of a recent survey study [ ], wherein the authors did not find any substantial differences in behavioral intentions between participants from rural and urban regions in China. Fourth, our dependent variables were only measured with one item that used a visual analog scale. This was done to keep the survey concise and specific. Unfortunately, validated measures such as the COVID-19–Induced Anxiety Scale or the Protective Behaviors Towards COVID-19 Scale [ ] were not available at the time of our survey. According to Heller et al [ ] and Price et al [ ], visual analog scales have sufficient psychometric measurement properties. Thus, they can be used when no validated instrument is available. Fifth, although the fitness of our path model was acceptable, it could have been better. However, it should be noted that as the sample size increases and the degrees of freedom remain constant, the Chi-square value increases. This leads to the problem of plausible models being rejected due to a significant Chi-square value. Therefore, too much emphasis should not be placed on the significance of the Chi-square statistic [ ]. Furthermore, it should be noted that our data are cross-sectional in nature. As such, causal conclusions cannot be drawn from our data. Future studies should be longitudinal in nature.Conclusions
Our results provide further insight into affective reactions and anticipated health-related behaviors during the COVID-19 pandemic. Our findings indicate that OCEAN personality traits are not associated with affective reactions and anticipated behaviors. Therefore, specific distinctions do not seem necessary when designing messages about health risks and health protection measures (ie, those related to hospital and medical practices during the COVID-19 pandemic). Even though future research is needed to confirm our results, health care workers should address the issues of patients with anxiety seriously and directly. Clear communication is necessary when providing information on the specific actions that hospitals and medical organizations perform to protect patients and health care workers. This could also help with preventing the cancellation of nonurgent surgeries in hospitals.
Authors' Contributions
TO, JR, and JG conceptualized this study. TO and TR designed the methodology of this study. JG, JR, and TO designed the survey. TO and TR performed the statistical analysis. TO, JG, and TR prepared the data. TO and TR wrote the initial manuscript draft. TO, JR, JG, and TR reviewed and edited the manuscript. TR created the figures and tables. TO supervised this study. All authors read and approved the published version of the manuscript.
Conflicts of Interest
None declared.
References
- RKI - Homepage. Robert Koch Institut. URL: https://www.rki.de/EN/Home/homepage_node.html [accessed 2021-02-23]
- Checklist for hospitals preparing for the reception and care of coronavirus 2019 (COVID-19) patients. European Centre for Disease Prevention and Control. 2020 Feb. URL: https://www.ecdc.europa.eu/sites/default/files/documents/covid-19-checklist-hospitals-preparing-reception-care-coronavirus-patients.pdf [accessed 2021-02-23]
- Boškoski I, Gallo C, Wallace MB, Costamagna G. COVID-19 pandemic and personal protective equipment shortage: protective efficacy comparing masks and scientific methods for respirator reuse. Gastrointest Endosc 2020 Sep;92(3):519-523 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Jordan RE, Adab P, Cheng KK. Covid-19: risk factors for severe disease and death. BMJ 2020 Mar 26;368:m1198. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Khosravi M. Perceived risk of COVID-19 pandemic: The role of public worry and trust. Electronic Journal of General Medicine 2020 Mar 27;17(4):em203 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef]
- Wang C, Pan R, Wan X, Tan Y, Xu L, Ho CS, et al. Immediate psychological responses and associated factors during the initial stage of the 2019 coronavirus disease (COVID-19) epidemic among the general population in China. Int J Environ Res Public Health 2020 Mar 06;17(5):1729 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Goodwin R, Gaines SO, Myers L, Neto F. Initial psychological responses to swine flu. Int J Behav Med 2011 Jun;18(2):88-92 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Meyer TJ, Miller ML, Metzger RL, Borkovec TD. Development and validation of the Penn State Worry Questionnaire. Behav Res Ther 1990;28(6):487-495. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Klein WMP, Zajac LE, Monin MM. Worry as a moderator of the association between risk perceptions and quitting intentions in young adult and adult smokers. Ann Behav Med 2009 Dec;38(3):256-261. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Muris P, Roelofs J, Rassin E, Franken I, Mayer B. Mediating effects of rumination and worry on the links between neuroticism, anxiety and depression. Pers Individ Dif 2005 Oct;39(6):1105-1111. [CrossRef]
- Campi R, Tellini R, Grosso AA, Amparore D, Mari A, Viola L, et al. Deferring elective urologic surgery during the COVID-19 pandemic: The patients' perspective. Urology 2021 Jan;147:21-26 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Chang J, Wignadasan W, Kontoghiorghe C, Kayani B, Singh S, Plastow R, et al. Restarting elective orthopaedic services during the COVID-19 pandemic: Do patients want to have surgery? Bone Jt Open 2020 Nov 01;1(6):267-271 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Binder S, Nuscheler R. Risk-taking in vaccination, surgery, and gambling environments: Evidence from a framed laboratory experiment. Health Econ 2017 Dec;26 Suppl 3:76-96. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Orom H, Penner LA, West BT, Downs TM, Rayford W, Underwood W. Personality predicts prostate cancer treatment decision-making difficulty and satisfaction. Psychooncology 2009 Mar;18(3):290-299 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Holmes BJ. Communicating about emerging infectious disease: The importance of research. Health, Risk & Society 2008 Sep 05;10(4):349-360 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef]
- Vaughan E, Tinker T. Effective health risk communication about pandemic influenza for vulnerable populations. Am J Public Health 2009 Oct;99 Suppl 2(Suppl 2):S324-S332. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Clement ND, Oussedik S, Raza KI, Patton RFL, Smith K, Deehan DJ. The rate of patient deferral and barriers to going forward with elective orthopaedic surgery during the COVID-19 pandemic. Bone Jt Open 2020 Oct 21;1(10):663-668 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- American Psychological Association. Revision of Ethical Standard 3.04 of the "Ethical Principles of Psychologists and Code of Conduct" (2002, as amended 2010). Am Psychol 2016 Dec;71(9):900-900. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Chenneville T, Schwartz-Mette R. Ethical considerations for psychologists in the time of COVID-19. Am Psychol 2020;75(5):644-654. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Marinaci T, Carpinelli L, Venuleo C, Savarese G, Cavallo P. Emotional distress, psychosomatic symptoms and their relationship with institutional responses: A survey of Italian frontline medical staff during the Covid-19 pandemic. Heliyon 2020 Dec 16;6(12):e05766 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Rammstedt B, John OP. Measuring personality in one minute or less: A 10-item short version of the Big Five Inventory in English and German. J Res Pers 2007 Feb;41(1):203-212. [CrossRef]
- Andresen B. HPI: Hamburger Persönlichkeitsinventar. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe; 2002.
- Laux L, Glanzmann P, Schaffner P, Spielberger C. Das State-Trait-Angstinventar. Theoretische Grundlagen und Handanweisung. Weinheim: Beltz Test GmbH; 1981.
- Li J, Huang DQ, Zou B, Yang H, Hui WZ, Rui F, et al. Epidemiology of COVID-19: A systematic review and meta-analysis of clinical characteristics, risk factors, and outcomes. J Med Virol 2021 Mar;93(3):1449-1458 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Altizer KP, Grzywacz JG, Quandt SA, Bell R, Arcury TA. A qualitative analysis of how elders seek and disseminate health information. Gerontol Geriatr Educ 2014;35(4):337-353 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Moreno A, Fuentes-Lara C, Navarro C. Covid-19 communication management in Spain: Exploring the effect of information-seeking behavior and message reception in public’s evaluation. El Profesional de la Información 2020 May 29;29(4):e290402 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef]
- Wang J, Wang X. Structural Equation Modeling: Applications Using Mplus, Second Edition. Hoboken, New Jersey, United States: John Wiley & Sons Ltd; Oct 29, 2019.
- Bollen KA, Long JS. Testing Structural Equation Models. Newbury Park: Sage Publications; Mar 1995.
- Muthén BO, Muthén LK, Asparouhov T. Regression and Mediation Analysis Using Mplus. Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén; 2016.
- Reiss S, Franchina V, Jutzi C, Willardt R, Jonas E. From anxiety to action-Experience of threat, emotional states, reactance, and action preferences in the early days of COVID-19 self-isolation in Germany and Austria. PLoS One 2020 Dec 8;15(12):e0243193. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Martin C. HEXACO traits, Big Five traits, and COVID-19. PsyArXiv. Preprint posted online on May 20, 2020 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef]
- Mitchell LL, Zmora R, Finlay JM, Jutkowitz E, Gaugler JE. Do Big Five personality traits moderate the effects of stressful life events on health trajectories? Evidence from the health and retirement study. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 2021 Jan 01;76(1):44-55. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Aschwanden D, Strickhouser JE, Sesker AA, Lee JH, Luchetti M, Stephan Y, et al. Psychological and behavioural responses to coronavirus disease 2019: The role of personality. Eur J Pers. Epub ahead of print 2020 Jul 08 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Walsh K, Campbell S, Ashby M, Procter S. Public anxiety and health policy: A psychodynamic perspective. Soc Theory Health 2016 Oct 19;14:493-509 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef]
- Aggleton P, Kippax S. Australia's HIV-prevention response: Introduction to the special issue. AIDS Educ Prev 2014 Jun;26(3):187-190. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Bults M, Beaujean DJ, de Zwart O, Kok G, van Empelen P, van Steenbergen JE, et al. Perceived risk, anxiety, and behavioural responses of the general public during the early phase of the Influenza A (H1N1) pandemic in the Netherlands: results of three consecutive online surveys. BMC Public Health 2011 Jan 03;11:2 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Jones JH, Salathé M. Early assessment of anxiety and behavioral response to novel swine-origin influenza A(H1N1). PLoS One 2009 Dec 03;4(12):e8032. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Rubin GJ, Amlôt R, Page L, Wessely S. Public perceptions, anxiety, and behaviour change in relation to the swine flu outbreak: cross sectional telephone survey. BMJ 2009 Jul 02;339:b2651 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Fotaki M, Hyde P. Organizational blind spots: Splitting, blame and idealization in the National Health Service. Hum Relat 2014 Jun 19;68(3):441-462. [CrossRef]
- Bluro IM, Piñeiro DJ, Estada JLN. 'Cardiovascular Pandemic' in Argentina. Glob Heart 2020 Jul 30;15(1):51. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Venkatesh A, Edirappuli S. Social distancing in covid-19: what are the mental health implications? BMJ 2020 Apr 06;369:m1379. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Ek S. Gender differences in health information behaviour: a Finnish population-based survey. Health Promot Int 2015 Sep;30(3):736-745. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Plümper T, Neumayer E. The pandemic predominantly hits poor neighbourhoods? SARS-CoV-2 infections and COVID-19 fatalities in German districts. Eur J Public Health 2020 Dec 11;30(6):1176-1180 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Chen X, Chen H. Differences in preventive behaviors of COVID-19 between urban and rural residents: Lessons learned from a cross-sectional study in China. Int J Environ Res Public Health 2020 Jun 20;17(12):4437 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Riad A, Huang Y, Zheng L, Elavsky S. COVID-19 induced anxiety and protective behaviors during COVID-19 outbreak: Scale development and validation. medRxiv. Preprint posted online on May 9, 2020 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef]
- Heller GX, Manuguerra M, Chow R. How to analyze the Visual Analogue Scale: Myths, truths and clinical relevance. Scand J Pain 2016 Oct;13:67-75. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Price DD, Staud R, Robinson ME. How should we use the visual analogue scale (VAS) in rehabilitation outcomes? II: Visual analogue scales as ratio scales: an alternative to the view of Kersten et al. J Rehabil Med 2012 Sep;44(9):800-801 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Schermelleh-Engel K, Moosbruger H, Müller H. Evaluating the fit of structural equation models: Tests of significance and descriptive goodness-of-fit measures. Methods of Psychological Research 2003;8(2):23-74.
Abbreviations
BFI: Big Five Inventory |
HPI: Hamburger Personality Inventory |
OCEAN: openness to experience, conscientiousness, extraversion, agreeableness, and neuroticism |
STAI: State-Trait Anxiety Inventory |
Edited by G Eysenbach; submitted 06.10.20; peer-reviewed by Z Ren, P Nguyen, P Le-Mai; comments to author 22.10.20; revised version received 07.01.21; accepted 21.01.21; published 11.03.21
Copyright©Thomas Ostermann, Julia Gampe, Jan Philipp Röer, Theda Radtke. Originally published in the Journal of Medical Internet Research (http://www.jmir.org), 11.03.2021.
This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work, first published in the Journal of Medical Internet Research, is properly cited. The complete bibliographic information, a link to the original publication on http://www.jmir.org/, as well as this copyright and license information must be included.