Published on in Vol 23, No 10 (2021): October

Preprints (earlier versions) of this paper are available at https://preprints.jmir.org/preprint/26810, first published .
Impulsivity, Self-control, Interpersonal Influences, and Maladaptive Cognitions as Factors of Internet Gaming Disorder Among Adolescents in China: Cross-sectional Mediation Study

Impulsivity, Self-control, Interpersonal Influences, and Maladaptive Cognitions as Factors of Internet Gaming Disorder Among Adolescents in China: Cross-sectional Mediation Study

Impulsivity, Self-control, Interpersonal Influences, and Maladaptive Cognitions as Factors of Internet Gaming Disorder Among Adolescents in China: Cross-sectional Mediation Study

Original Paper

1Center for Health Behaviours Research, Jockey Club School of Public Health and Primary Care, The Chinese University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong, Hong Kong

2West China School of Public Health, Sichuan University, Chengdu, China

3Department of Clinical Research, State Key Laboratory of Oncology in South China, Collaborative Innovation Center for Cancer Medicine, Sun Yat-sen University Cancer Center, Guangzhou, China

Corresponding Author:

Joseph Tak-Fai Lau, PhD

Center for Health Behaviours Research

Jockey Club School of Public Health and Primary Care

The Chinese University of Hong Kong

Ngan Shing St

Sha Tin

Hong Kong

Hong Kong

Phone: 852 2252 8713

Email: jlau@cuhk.edu.hk


Background: Gaming disorder, including internet gaming disorder (IGD), was recently defined by the World Health Organization as a mental disease in the 11th Revision of the International Classification of Diseases (ICD-11). Thus, reducing IGD is warranted. Maladaptive cognitions related to internet gaming (MCIG) have been associated with IGD, while impulsivity, self-control, parental influences, and peer influences are key risk factors of IGD. Previous literature suggests that MCIG is associated with the aforementioned 4 risk factors and IGD, and may thus mediate between these risk factors and IGD. These potential mediations, if significant, imply that modification of MCIG may possibly alleviate these risk factors’ harmful impacts on increasing IGD. These mediation hypotheses were tested in this study for the first time.

Objective: This study tested the mediation effects of MCIG between intrapersonal factors (impulsivity and self-control) and IGD, and between interpersonal factors (parental influences and peer influences) and IGD among adolescents in China.

Methods: An anonymous, cross-sectional, and self-administered survey was conducted among secondary school students in classroom settings in Guangzhou and Chengdu, China. All grade 7 to 9 students (7 to 9 years of formal education) of 7 secondary schools were invited to join the study, and 3087 completed the survey. The Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5) checklist was used to assess IGD. MCIG was assessed by using the Chinese version of the Revised Internet Gaming Cognition Scale. Impulsivity, self-control, and parental or peer influences were measured by using the motor subscale of the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale, the Brief Self-Control Scale, and the modified interpersonal influence scale, respectively. Structural equation modeling was conducted to examine the mediation effects of MCIG between these risk factors and IGD.

Results: The prevalence of IGD was 13.57% (418/3081) and 17.67% (366/2071) among all participants and adolescent internet gamers, respectively. The 3 types of MCIG (perceived rewards of internet gaming, perceived urges for playing internet games, and perceived unwillingness to stop playing without completion of gaming tasks) were positively associated with IGD. Impulsivity, self-control, parental influences, and peer influences were all significantly associated with the 3 types of MCIG and IGD. The 3 types of MCIG partially mediated the associations between the studied factors and IGD (effect size of 30.0% to 37.8%).

Conclusions: Impulsivity, self-control, and interpersonal influences had both direct and indirect effects via MCIG on IGD. Modifications of the 3 types of MCIG can potentially reduce the harmful impacts of impulsivity and interpersonal influences on IGD and enhance the protective effect of self-control against IGD. Future longitudinal studies are warranted.

J Med Internet Res 2021;23(10):e26810

doi:10.2196/26810

Keywords



After the inclusion of internet gaming disorder (IGD) in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition (DSM-5) in 2013 [1], World Health Organization (WHO) recognized gaming disorder (online and offline) as a disease in the 11th Revision of International Classification of Diseases (ICD-11) in 2019 [2]. The reported range of prevalence of IGD among Chinese adolescents is wide (2.4% to 21.5%), possibly due to methodological differences [3]. Adolescents are vulnerable to IGD, which has many adverse consequences (eg, loneliness and depression [4-6]).

Maladaptive cognition is an important intrapersonal factor of addictive behaviors like pathological gambling [7] and internet addiction [8]; maladaptive cognitions related to internet gaming (MCIG) have also been associated with IGD [8-10]. A systematic review of 36 studies of MCIG proposed a 4-factor cognitive framework (ie, overvaluation of gaming rewards, maladaptive rules, gaming for self-esteem, and gaming for social acceptance) [11]; the 4 types of MCIG were all positively associated with IGD among adolescents [9]. A recent validation study modified this 4-factor model and revealed a new 3-factor model (ie, perceived rewards of internet gaming, perceived urges for playing internet games, and perceived unwillingness to stop playing without completion of gaming tasks) that demonstrated satisfactory psychometric properties [12]. The 3 domains of MCIG were all positively correlated with IGD [12]. The revised scale was used in this study.

Impulsivity and self-control are 2 important intrapersonal factors of addictive behaviors, including substance use [13,14], smoking [15], alcohol drinking [16], internet addiction [17], and IGD [4,18-21]. Impulsivity and self-control fit into the dual-process theories of decision-making for risk behaviors [22-25]. Impulsivity represents reactive, intuitive, and affective processes with high responsiveness to temptations and prompt actions without deliberation [24]. In contrast, self-control reflects the reasoned process and ability in regulating impulses deliberatively [26,27]. The 2 processes jointly affect decisions in the performance of risk behaviors [22-25]. Adolescents with high impulsivity may exhibit heightened spontaneous responses to behavioral cues to internet gaming, while those with low self-control may find it difficult to resist the temptation of playing internet games and stop playing.

Interpersonal influences are important factors of IGD. Empirical studies have reported positive associations between interpersonal influences (eg, parents’ invitations of playing internet games and intensity of peers’ gaming behaviors) and IGD or internet addiction among high schools students [28,29]. Interpersonal influences may affect IGD in different ways. Significant others’ direct invitations to play internet games may trigger prompt engagement in internet gaming. According to the health belief model, such invitations to play internet games represent cues to action, which is an important determinant of health-related behavior [30]. Frequent invitations may increase adolescents’ gaming intensity, which is associated with risk of IGD [28]. In addition, the reciprocal determinism construct of social cognitive theory (SCT) postulates that one’s environment, personal factors, and health-related behaviors interact with each other [31]. Having significant others playing internet games frequently forms a social environment that may increase adolescents’ gaming frequency. More importantly, SCT postulates that observational learning is an important factor of health-related behavior [31]; adolescents may play internet games frequently through observational learning from their significant others’ frequent internet gaming.

Importantly, MCIG are potential mediators of the association between impulsivity or self-control and IGD. Brand’s model [32] proposes that dysfunctional personality traits increase addictiveness to specific internet applications (including internet gaming), through mediation of changes in related cognitions (eg, expectations). Cognition of outcome expectancy regarding addictive behaviors has been shown to partially mediate the associations between impulsivity (a type of dysfunctional personality trait) and addictive behavior (eg, substance use and alcohol drinking) [33,34]. These studies, however, did not look at IGD. Following Brand’s model, this study hence tested whether MCIG would mediate the association between impulsivity and IGD. This contention is indirectly supported by a study that reported a partial mediation of the association between maladaptive personality traits (ie, negative affectivity, detachment, and psychoticism) and IGD via the cognition of expectancies related to using internet gaming to escape from reality [35]. That study, however, did not include other types of MCIG. Furthermore, we did not locate studies that looked at MCIG as a mediator between self-control and IGD. This study thus sought to fill some of deficiencies in the relevant literature.

The mediation between interpersonal influences and IGD via MCIG was also tested in this study. The hypothesis is supported by the theory of triadic influence, which is a framework that incorporates influential factors of addictive behaviors from a number of psychological and sociological theories [36]. It distinguishes between distal or ultimate factors that influence proximal factors of changes in health-related behaviors [37]. According to the theory of triadic influence, interpersonal factors are distal or ultimate variables while cognitive factors related to a certain behavior are proximal variables [36]; the theory suggests that cognitive factors (eg, outcome expectancy) mediate between interpersonal influences and health-related behaviors [38,39]. Such mediations were found for some addictive behaviors (eg, substance use [40], alcohol drinking [41], and internet addiction [29]). One study also reported full and partial mediation of the cognition of positive outcome expectancy of internet gaming between interpersonal influences (peers’ positive attitude toward gaming, intensity of peers’ internet game use, and the frequency of peers’ invitation to play internet games) and IGD among high school students [28]. To our knowledge, however, no study has looked at the mediation effects of other types of MCIG.

We thus investigated the factors of IGD, including impulsivity, self-control, interpersonal influences exerted by parents and peers, and the 3 types of MCIG (perceived rewards, perceived urges, and perceived unwillingness to stop playing) among junior middle school students (grade 7 to 9 of formal education) in 2 populous cities in China. We then tested the mediation effects of MCIG on the associations between impulsivity or self-control and IGD, and between interpersonal influences and IGD, respectively. We hypothesized that the direct and indirect effects would be statistically significant.


Participants and Data Collection

The cross-sectional survey was conducted among secondary school students in Guangzhou and Chengdu in China from October 2018 to December of 2018. The 2 cities are located in southern and southwestern China, which had populations of 14.9 and 16.3 million people in 2018, respectively, and 0.36 and 0.40 million secondary school students in 2018, respectively [42]. Seven junior middle schools (4 out of 409 from Guangzhou and 3 out of 460 from Chengdu) were conveniently selected and participated in the study. All the Grade 7 students (7 years of formal education) in Guangzhou and all the Grade 7 to 9 students in Chengdu were invited to join the survey. The inclusion criteria were those who were full-time Grade 7 students and Grade 7-9 students of the participating schools in Guangzhou and Chengdu, respectively; and those willing to participate in the study. The procedure of data collection was described in a published study that used a subsample of the survey [12], and it is briefly introduced here. Under the supervision of well-trained field workers, students self-administered an anonymous structured questionnaire in the absence of teachers in classroom settings. They were briefed about the objectives of the survey, the return of completed questionnaires implied informed consent, and the students had the right to quit at any time without any negative consequences. No incentives were given to the students. The study was approved by the Survey and Behavioral Research Ethics Committee of the Chinese University of Hong Kong (#SBRE-18-430).

Of the 4350 students invited to participate in the present study, 3147 (72.34%) returned the questionnaire. Among all the returned questionnaires, 66 (2.10%) were removed from data analyses as there were more than 20% of missing data in their response items. Data obtained from the remaining 3081 students (97.90%) were used for data analysis (1126 from Guangzhou [36.55%] and 1955 from Chengdu [63.45%]).

Measures

Background Variables

Background information was collected, including sex, grades, being born in the studied city, whether living with both parents, both father’s and mother’s educational levels (junior middle school or below, senior middle school or equal, or college or above), household income level compared with their classmates (5 points: much higher to much lower), and self-reported academic performance (3 points: above average, average, and below average).

IGD Assessment

The 9-item DSM-5 checklist was used to assess IGD [43]; it recorded the presence of addictive symptoms, including preoccupation, withdrawal, tolerance, inability to control internet gaming, loss of interest in other activities, psychological or social problems, deception, avoidance, and significant loss due to internet gaming. IGD is defined by endorsement of ≥ 5 items (yes-no response options). The Chinese version of DSM-5 has been validated as having good psychometric properties and diagnostic validity [44,45]. The Cronbach α of the checklist was .79 in the present study.

Maladaptive Cognitions Related to Internet Gaming

MCIG was measured by using the Chinese version of the Revised Internet Gaming Cognition Scale (C-RIGCS). It consists of 3 subscales: perceived rewards of internet gaming, perceived urges for playing internet games, and perceived unwillingness to stop playing without completion of gaming tasks. Sample items are “I feel more in control when I play internet games,” “I would feel bad if I was not able to play internet games,” and “I feel uncomfortable thinking about my unfinished goals or objectives in internet games.” The C-RIGCS has been validated in Chinese adolescents and has shown acceptable psychometric properties [12]. The items were rated with 5-point Likert scales (0=never to 4=always), with higher scores indicating higher levels of MCIG. The Cronbach α of the overall scale and its 3 subscales in the present study were .91, .86, .81, and .74, respectively.

Impulsivity

Impulsivity was measured by using the 10-item motor impulsiveness subscale of the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale, which indicates the tendency to act on the spur of the moment and with fast reactions [46]. The Chinese version made some cultural adaptations and showed good reliability and construct validity in Chinese adolescents [47]. A sample item is “I do things without thinking.” The items were rated with 5-point Likert scales (1=completely disagree to 5=completely agree), with higher scores indicating higher levels of impulsivity. The Cronbach α of the scale was .91 in this study.

Self-Control

Self-control was measured by using the 13-item Brief Self-Control Scale [26], which demonstrated good psychometric properties in Chinese adolescents [48]. A sample item is “I am good at resisting temptation.” The items were rated with 5-point Likert scales (1=never to 5=always), with higher scores indicating higher levels of self-control. The Cronbach α of the scale was .74 in this study.

Interpersonal Influences

Interpersonal influences were measured by revising the 6 items that assessed similar tendencies in a previous study [28]. The items included frequency of being invited to play internet games from parents and peers, perceived parents’ and peers’ gaming intensity, and perceived parental influences and peer influences on current internet gaming behavior. Sample items were “How often do your parents invite you to play internet games?”, “How often do your parents play internet games?”, and “To what extent do you think your parents affect your internet gaming behavior?” Confirmatory factor analysis was conducted to examine the 2-factor structure (parental influences and peer influences) of the 6 items, which showed an acceptable goodness of fit (comparative fit index [CFI]=0.97, Tucker-Lewis index [TLI]=0.97, and root mean square error of approximation [RMSEA]=0.09). The items were rated with 4-point Likert scales (1=never/nil to 4=always/severe), with higher scores indicating higher levels of interpersonal influences. The Cronbach α of the overall scale and its 2 subscales in this study were .73, .63 (a Cronbach α >.60 was considered acceptable in previous literature [49,50]), and .71, respectively.

Statistical Analysis

IGD was used as the binary dependent variable. Univariate logistic regression analysis was conducted to establish the associations between the studied background variables and IGD; crude odds ratios (ORcs) and their respective 95% CIs were derived. Pearson correlation coefficients (rp) and Spearman correlation coefficients (rs) were derived for continuous and ordinal variables, respectively. The mediation effects were tested by using structural equation modeling (SEM) with weighted least square mean and variance-adjusted estimation. Three latent variables were created: (1) impulsivity or low self-control was derived from the scale scores of impulsivity and self-control (reversed scores), (2) interpersonal influences was derived from the subscale scores of parental influences and peer influences, and (3) maladaptive cognitions was derived from the subscale scores of the 3 types of MCIG. The paths between the 3 latent variables and IGD were fit to test the mediation hypotheses. Recommended goodness-of-fit indicators included CFI ≥0.90, TLI ≥0.90, and RMSEA ≤0.08. The SEM was conducted by using Mplus 7.0; other statistical analyses were performed with SPSS version 21.0 (IBM Corp). Statistical significance was defined as a 2-tailed P value <.05.


Descriptive Statistics

More than half of the participants were males (1550/3081, 50.31%) and first-year students (1979/3081, 64.23%). More than one-fifth were not born in the city where the study was conducted (691/3081, 22.43%) and did not live with both parents (639/3081, 20.74%). Around one-fifth of the participants’ fathers (594/3081, 19.28%) and mothers (566/3081, 18.37%) had received tertiary education or above; 12.56% (387/3081) self-perceived a lower or much lower household income level than did their classmates, and 19.47% (600/3081) self-reported a below-average academic performance (see Table 1). The mean of impulsivity, self-control, parental influences, and peer influences were 22.9 (SD 7.6, range 10-50), 44.4 (SD 7.6, range 13-65), 4.8 (SD 1.8, range 3-12), and 6.5 (SD 2.1, range 3-12), respectively. Similarly, the mean of the overall C-RIGCS and its 3 subscales were 17.0 (SD 11.6, range 0-60), 7.2 (SD 5.9, range 0-28), 3.7 (SD 3.5, range 0-16), and 6.1 (SD 3.7, range 0-16), respectively.

Table 1. Background characteristics of participants (N=3081).
CharacteristicValue, n (%)
Sex

Female1525 (49.49)

Male1550 (50.31)

Missing data6 (0.19)
Grade

Seven1979 (64.23)

Eight579 (18.79)

Nine523 (16.98)
Study site

Guangzhou1126 (36.55)

Chengdu1955 (63.45)
Born in the city where the study was conducted

Yes2367 (76.83)

No691 (22.43)

Missing data23 (0.75)
Living with both parents

Yes2382 (77.31)

No639 (20.74)

Missing data60 (1.95)
Father’s educational level

Junior middle school or below1489 (48.33)

Senior middle school or equal831 (26.97)

College or above594 (19.28)

Missing data167 (5.42)
Mother’s educational level

Junior middle school or below1532 (49.72)

Senior middle school or equal803 (26.06)

College or above566 (18.37)

Missing data180 (5.84)
Household income level when compared with classmates

Much higher/higher672 (21.81)

Moderate2002 (64.98)

Lower/much lower387 (12.56)

Missing data20 (0.65)
Self-reported academic performance

Above average1020 (33.11)

Average1348 (43.75)

Below average600 (19.47)

Missing data113 (3.67)

Prevalence of IGD

The prevalence of IGD was 13.57% (418/3081; 95% CI 12.4%-14.5%) among all participants. Among those who had played internet games in the past 12 months (2071/3081, 67.22% of all participants), the prevalence of IGD was 17.67% (366/2071; 95% CI 16.0%-19.3%).

Associations Between Background Variables and IGD

The univariate logistic regression analyses showed that the background variables were all significantly associated with IGD, except for place of birth (whether born in the city where the study was conducted; Table 2). Significant factors included sex (males vs females: ORc=2.80, 95% CI 2.23-3.51), student grade (Grade 8 vs 7: ORc=1.90, 95% CI 1.49-2.44; Grade 9 vs 7: ORc=1.36, 95% CI 1.03-1.80), study site (Chengdu vs Guangzhou: ORc=2.12, 95% CI 1.66-2.69), living arrangement (not living vs living with both parents: ORc=1.54, 95% CI 1.22-1.95), household income level (self-perceived lower or much lower vs higher or much higher than other classmates: ORc=1.64, 95% CI 1.17-2.30), parental education (father’s tertiary vs primary education or below: ORc=0.53, 95% CI 0.39-0.73; mothers’ tertiary vs primary education or below: ORc=0.72, 95% CI 0.53-0.97), and self-reported academic performance (average vs above average: ORc=1.35, 95% CI 1.04-1.76; below average vs above average: ORc=2.53, 95% CI 1.90-3.36).

Table 2. Univariate logistic regression analysis on the associations between the studied background variables and internet gaming disorder (N=3081).
Background variablesIGDa, n (%)Association, ORcb (95% CI)
Sex

Femalec119 (7.8)N/Ad

Male297 (19.16)2.80 (2.23-3.51)***
Grade

Sevenc226 (11.42)N/A

Eight114 (19.69)1.90 (1.49-2.44)***

Nine78 (14.91)1.36 (1.03-1.80)*
Study site

Guangzhouc96 (8.53)N/A

Chengdu322 (16.47)2.12 (1.66-2.69)***
Born in the city where the study was conductede

Yesc308 (13.01)N/A

No105 (15.2)1.20 (0.94-1.52)
Living with both parentse

Yesc294 (12.34)

No114 (17.84)1.54 (1.22-1.95)***
Father’s educational levele

Junior middle school or belowc227 (15.25)N/A

Senior middle school or equal109 (13.12)0.84 (0.66-1.07)

College or above52 (8.75)0.53 (0.39-0.73)***
Mother’s educational levele

Junior middle school or belowc218 (14.23)N/A

Senior middle school or equal102 (12.7)0.88 (0.68-1.13)

College or above60 (10.6)0.72 (0.53-0.97)*
Household income level when compared with classmatese

Much higher/higherc87 (12.95)N/A

Moderate246 (12.29)0.94 (0.73-1.22)

Lower/much lower76 (19.64)1.64 (1.17-2.30)**
Self-reported academic performancee

Above averagec98 (9.61)N/A

Average169 (12.54)1.35 (1.04-1.76)*

Below average127 (21.17)2.53 (1.90-3.36)***

aIGD: internet gaming disorder.

bORc: crude odds ratio.

cReference=1.0

dN/A: not applicable.

eMissing data were excluded from the analysis.

*P<.05.

**P<.01.

***P<.001.

Correlations Among the Studied Variables

The 3 studied risk factors (impulsivity, parental influences, and peer influences) were all positively correlated with the overall C-RIGCS and its 3 subscales representing MCIG (rp ranged from 0.19 to 0.39; P<.001) and IGD (rs ranged from 0.24 to 0.26; P<.001), respectively. Self-control was negatively correlated with the overall C-RIGCS and its 3 subscales (rp ranged from –0.45 to –0.27; P<.001) and IGD (rs=–0.32; P<.001), respectively. Besides the above correlations, all the studied factors of IGD were significantly correlated with each other (see Table 3).

Table 3. Correlations among impulsivity, self-control, interpersonal influences, maladaptive cognitions, and internet gaming disorder (N=3081)a.
Major variable123456789
1. Impulsivitybc







2. Self-control0.58*






3. Parental influencesb0.26*0.24*





4. Peer influencesb0.24*0.21*0.40*




5. Overall maladaptive cognitionsb0.37*0.38*0.30*0.39*



6. Perceived rewards of internet gamingb0.31*0.32*0.29*0.37*0.93*


7. Perceived urges for playing internet gamesb0.39*0.45*0.28*0.35*0.85*0.69*

8. Perceived unwillingness to stop playing without completion of gaming tasksb0.28*0.27*0.19*0.30*0.85*0.68*0.61*
9. IGDd,e0.30*0.41*0.37*0.42*0.24*0.26*0.26*0.32*

aMissing data were excluded from the analyses.

bPearson correlation analyses.

cNot applicable.

dSpearman correlation analyses.

eIGD: internet gaming disorder.

*P<.001.

The SEM Model Testing the Mediation Hypotheses

Figure 1 presents the SEM model that demonstrated a satisfactory model fit (CFI=0.95, TLI=0.90, and RMSEA=0.08); the factor loadings of the 3 latent variables ranged from 0.57 to 0.89 (all P<.001). The findings revealed that maladaptive cognitions partially mediated the association between impulsivity or low self-control and IGD (mediation effect size=30.0%; P of Sobel test <.001), and between interpersonal influences and IGD (mediation effect size=37.8%; P of Sobel test <.001), respectively. Impulsivity or low self-control (standardized β=.29; P<.001) and interpersonal influences (standardized β=.24; P<.001) had significant direct effects on IGD. The standardized β values of the other paths are presented in Figure 1.

Figure 1. The structural equation model testing the mediation hypotheses. IGD: internet gaming disorder. *P<.001.
View this figure

This study revealed an alarmingly high prevalence of IGD (around 14% among all adolescent participants and 18% among adolescent internet gamers). The prevalence was comparable to the 13% among adolescents of 2 other Chinese cities [51], but much higher than the 2% among Chinese Macau adults [52] and the 3.1% among Australian adolescents [9]. These studies all used DSM-5 criteria to assess IGD. The high prevalence of IGD signifies the need to regulate internet gaming behaviors and conduct interventions to reduce IGD among adolescents in China, as adolescent IGD has been positively associated with various behavioral problems (eg, aggression and violence [53,54]) and mental health problems (eg, loneliness and depression [4-6]).

In our study, a number of background factors were associated with a higher risk of IGD, including male sex, higher grades, not living with both parents, and self-reported below-average academic performance. First, corroborating previous studies [18,55,56], males were associated with a higher risk of IGD. This finding may be explained by the sex differences in brain responses [57], motives of internet gaming [58], and a less female-friendly gaming environment [59], among other plausible factors. Second, higher grades were positively associated with IGD in this study. Similar findings have been reported [9], but the direction of the association between age and IGD was mixed [18,60]. Future longitudinal studies are warranted to examine the associations between sex, age, grade, and IGD to develop sex- and age-specific interventions to reduce IGD. Furthermore, disadvantaged adolescents, including those of lower socioeconomic status (eg, self-perceived lower household income) and those not living with both parents were more likely than others to have IGD. Similar results were reported in previous longitudinal and cross-sectional studies [61,62]. Disadvantaged adolescents might have fewer resources for leisure activities and less parental monitoring over internet use, which may lead to more intensive internet gaming and higher risk of developing IGD [61]. A negative association between academic performance and IGD was also reported in our study, which was consistent with previous literature [18,63,64]. It is worth noting that the relationship between academic performance and IGD may be bidirectional. Frustration over poor academic performance may lead to problematic gaming as a means of escape and maladaptive coping [65], which has been associated with IGD [66]. Conversely, IGD may lead to poor academic performance [5,67]. The causal direction needs to be confirmed by longitudinal studies.

To facilitate the design of effective interventions for reducing IGD, it is important to understand the mechanisms (mediation) underlying the associations between risk or protective factors of IGD and IGD. The findings reveal that some intrapersonal (impulsivity) and interpersonal (interpersonal influences) risk factors may elevate the levels of the 3 types of MCIG, which may in turn increase the risk of developing IGD. Furthermore, self-control may reduce MCIG, which may increase IGD (a partial mediation effect). Nonetheless, the presence of significant direct effects between impulsivity, self-control, and interpersonal influences and IGD imply the existence of other unstudied mediators. For instance, coping is a potential mediator, as Brand’s model postulates dysfunctional copying strategies mediate between personality traits and problematic use of the internet (including IGD) [32], while impulsivity can be considered a personality trait. Interpersonal influences may also strengthen the subjective norms of internet gaming (ie, significant others’ support for internet gaming), which is a construct of the theory of planned behaviors [68]; these subjective norms are expected to be associated with MCIG. Future studies should look at other mediators.

The observed mediation effects suggest that modifications of the 3 types of MCIG can potentially reduce the harmful impacts of impulsivity or interpersonal influences on IGD and increase the protective effect of self-control against IGD. Targeted interventions to improve MCIG may include training to increase awareness of the 3 types of MCIG and skills to perform related cognitive reconstructions [69], provision of alternative sources of rewards (eg, outdoors activities) to reduce perceived rewards of internet gaming, removal of sources of temptations and stimuli (eg, gaming devices), and introduction of distraction skills to reduce perceived urges or unwillingness to stop playing internet games [70,71].

There are also plausible explanations for why impulsivity was positively associated with the 3 types of MCIG. First, impulsivity may increase reward sensitivity that enhances adolescents’ drives to seeking more rewards from addictive behaviors [72,73]; impulsive adolescents may thus possess a higher reward drive and perceive more rewards from internet gaming. Second, impulsive adolescents may be more responsive to cues of internet gaming and thus hold stronger urges for playing. Third, impulsivity in general may reduce impulse inhibitions against addictive behaviors, even in the presence of negative consequences [74-76]; adolescents with weakened inhibitions of gaming impulses may thus be less able to resist stimuli inductive to playing internet games and may perceive stronger unwillingness to stop playing. Self-control was negatively associated with the 3 types of MCIG, possibly because of the negative association between impulsivity and self-control [22-25], but other reasons may also apply. Interventions for modifying impulsivity and self-control may remove temptations and strengthen self-efficacy in regulating impulses [77]). In particular, the if-then planning intervention that specifies when, where, and how to regulate impulses is potentially useful [78]; a review reported that this type of intervention showed efficacy in reducing addictive behaviors (eg, in binge drinking and cigarette smoking) [79].

This study also found positive associations between interpersonal influences and MCIG. Having significant others who are frequent internet gamers was positively associated with MCIG. The SCT suggests that, through reciprocal determinism and observational learning, social interactions may influence both adolescents’ attitudes and behavior [31]. It is likely that adolescents’ parents or peers who play internet games frequently may also perceive higher levels of MCIG (eg, the 3 types of MCIG). Adolescents’ maladaptive cognitions may be influenced by those of their parents via multiple means, such as social learning, subjective norms, and reinforcement. Furthermore, peers influence each other to form common perceptions regarding particular behaviors [80]. Future confirmation is needed. Family-based interventions for reducing IGD are potentially useful. A psychoeducation on maladaptive and adaptive use related to internet games showed preliminary effectiveness [81]. Another intervention that involved parental monitoring on adolescents’ self-regulation practice and gaming behaviors showed short-term (3 month) efficacy in improving attitude, knowledge, self-regulation, and IGD among adolescents [82]. Although a meta-analysis showed that peer-led interventions were efficacious in reducing tobacco, alcohol, and substance use among adolescents [83], our literature search did not locate similar interventions for IGD. Future evidence-based interventions are warranted.

This study has several limitations. First, reporting bias, such as recall bias and social desirability bias, might have been introduced. Second, as the participating schools were selected based on convenience, there might have been selection bias, and generalization of the study results should be done cautiously. Third, the prevalence of IGD assessed by using the DSM-5 checklist might have been overestimated compared with that based on the ICD-11 criteria [84]. Fourth, we were unable to make causal inferences due to the cross-sectional nature of this study. Fifth, the revised assessment tool of interpersonal influences has not been validated although we conducted confirmatory factor analysis to test its 2-factor structure, which showed an acceptable goodness of fit. Finally, this study only investigated internet gaming but not offline video games, while both online and offline video games are included in the ICD-11 under the category of gaming disorder, as the 2 types of gaming disorder differ in etiology, epidemiology, and treatment.

In conclusion, MCIG partially mediated the associations between impulsivity, self-control, or interpersonal influences and IGD. Modifications of the 3 types of MCIG may effectively reduce the harmful impacts of impulsivity or interpersonal influences on IGD and increase the protective effect of self-control against IGD. Future longitudinal studies are warranted to verify these findings and explore other potential mediators.

Acknowledgments

This research was funded by National Natural Science Foundation of China (grant #81373021). We would like to thank all the participants for their contribution.

Authors' Contributions

JTFL and YY conceived the study. YY, PKHM, and JTFL were responsible for the methodology. JZ and JL conducted the investigation. YY was responsible for the software and data curation, and conducted the formal analysis. PKHM and JTFL were responsible for validation. JZ, JL, and JTFL acquired the study resources. YY and JTFL wrote the original draft. YY, PKHM, and JTFL reviewed and edited the draft. JTFL conducted supervision. JL and JTFL acquired funding for the study.

Conflicts of Interest

None declared.

  1. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM–5). Arlington: American Psychiatric Association; 2013.
  2. World Health Organization. The Eleventh Revision of the International Classification of Diseases. Geneva: World Health Organization; 2018.
  3. Paulus FW, Ohmann S, von Gontard A, Popow C. Internet gaming disorder in children and adolescents: a systematic review. Dev Med Child Neurol 2018 Apr 06;60(7):645-659. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  4. Gentile DA, Choo H, Liau A, Sim T, Li D, Fung D, et al. Pathological video game use among youths: a two-year longitudinal study. PEDIATRICS 2011 Jan 17;127(2):e319-e329. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  5. Brunborg GS, Mentzoni RA, Frøyland LR. Is video gaming, or video game addiction, associated with depression, academic achievement, heavy episodic drinking, or conduct problems? Journal of Behavioral Addictions 2014 Mar;3(1):27-32. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  6. Liau AK, Choo H, Li D, Gentile DA, Sim T, Khoo A. Pathological video-gaming among youth: A prospective study examining dynamic protective factors. Addiction Research & Theory 2014 Dec 19;23(4):301-308. [CrossRef]
  7. Goodie AS, Fortune EE. Measuring cognitive distortions in pathological gambling: Review and meta-analyses. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors 2013 Sep;27(3):730-743. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  8. Davis R. A cognitive-behavioral model of pathological Internet use. Computers in Human Behavior 2001 Mar;17(2):187-195. [CrossRef]
  9. King DL, Delfabbro PH. The cognitive psychopathology of internet gaming disorder in adolescence. J Abnorm Child Psychol 2016 Feb 15;44(8):1635-1645. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  10. Walker MB. Irrational thinking among slot machine players. J Gambling Stud 1992;8(3):245-261. [CrossRef]
  11. King DL, Delfabbro PH. The cognitive psychology of Internet gaming disorder. Clinical Psychology Review 2014 Jun;34(4):298-308. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  12. Yu Y, Mo PK, Zhang J, Li J, Lau JT. Validation of the Chinese Version of the Revised Internet Gaming Cognition Scale among Adolescents in China: Maladaptive Cognitions as Potential Determinants of Internet Gaming Disorder. IJERPH 2019 Dec 31;17(1):290. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  13. Bolles JR, Earleywine M, Gordis E. Acquired preparedness model and impulsivity moderated expectancies: Examining predictors of marijuana use. Addiction Research & Theory 2014 Mar 05;22(6):490-497. [CrossRef]
  14. Gibbons FX, O'Hara RE, Stock ML, Gerrard M, Weng C, Wills TA. The erosive effects of racism: Reduced self-control mediates the relation between perceived racial discrimination and substance use in African American adolescents. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 2012;102(5):1089-1104. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  15. Kvaavik E, Rise J. How do impulsivity and education relate to smoking initiation and cessation among young adults? J. Stud. Alcohol Drugs 2012 Sep;73(5):804-810. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  16. LaBrie JW, Kenney SR, Napper LE, Miller K. Impulsivity and alcohol-related risk among college students: Examining urgency, sensation seeking and the moderating influence of beliefs about alcohol's role in the college experience. Addictive Behaviors 2014 Jan;39(1):159-164. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  17. Cao F, Su L, Liu T, Gao X. The relationship between impulsivity and Internet addiction in a sample of Chinese adolescents. Eur. psychiatr 2007 Aug 31;22(7):466-471. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  18. Wittek CT, Finserås TR, Pallesen S, Mentzoni RA, Hanss D, Griffiths MD, et al. Prevalence and predictors of video game addiction: a study based on a national representative sample of gamers. Int J Ment Health Addiction 2015 Sep 23;14(5):672-686. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  19. Kim EJ, Namkoong K, Ku T, Kim SJ. The relationship between online game addiction and aggression, self-control and narcissistic personality traits. Eur. psychiatr 2007 Dec 31;23(3):212-218. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  20. Choo H, Gentile DA, Sim T, Li D, Khoo A, Liau AK. Pathological video-gaming among Singaporean youth. Ann Acad Med Singap 2010 Nov;39(11):822-829 [FREE Full text] [Medline]
  21. Rehbein F, Psych G, Kleimann M, Mediasci G, Mößle T. Prevalence and risk factors of video game dependency in adolescence: results of a German nationwide survey. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking 2010 Jun;13(3):269-277. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  22. Halpern-Felsher B. Adolescent decision-making. Encyclopedia of Adolescence 2011;1:30-37. [CrossRef]
  23. Reyna VF, Farley F. Risk and rationality in adolescent decision making: implications for theory, practice, and public policy. Psychol Sci Public Interest 2016 Jun 23;7(1):1-44. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  24. Lieberman MD. Social cognitive neuroscience: a review of core processes. Annu. Rev. Psychol 2007 Jan;58(1):259-289. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  25. Cabeza R, Nyberg L. Neural bases of learning and memory: functional neuroimaging evidence. Current Opinion in Neurology 2000;13(4):415-421. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  26. Tangney JP, Baumeister RF, Boone AL. High self-control predicts good adjustment, less pathology, better grades, and interpersonal success. J Personality 2004 Apr;72(2):271-324. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  27. Finkenauer C, Engels RCME, Baumeister RF. Parenting behaviour and adolescent behavioural and emotional problems: The role of self-control. International Journal of Behavioral Development 2016 Jun 30;29(1):58-69. [CrossRef]
  28. Wu JYW, Ko H, Wong T, Wu L, Oei TP. Positive outcome expectancy mediates the relationship between peer influence and Internet gaming addiction among adolescents in Taiwan. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking 2016 Jan;19(1):49-55. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  29. Lin M, Wu JY, Chen C, You J. Positive outcome expectancy mediates the relationship between social influence and Internet addiction among senior high-school students. J Behav Addict 2018 Jun 27;7(2):292-300. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  30. Janz NK, Becker MH. The health belief model: a decade later. Health Education Quarterly 2016 Sep 04;11(1):1-47. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  31. Akers R. Deviant Behavior: A Social Learning Approach. 2nd ed. Belmont: Wadsworth; 1977.
  32. Brand M, Young KS, Laier C. Prefrontal control and Internet addiction: a theoretical model and review of neuropsychological and neuroimaging findings. Front. Hum. Neurosci 2014 May 27;8:375. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  33. Treloar HR, Morris DH, Pedersen SL, Mccarthy DM. Direct and indirect effects of impulsivity traits on drinking and driving in young adults. J. Stud. Alcohol Drugs 2012 Sep;73(5):794-803. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  34. Vangsness L, Bry BH, LaBouvie EW. Impulsivity, negative expectancies, and marijuana use: a test of the acquired preparedness model. Addictive Behaviors 2005 Jun;30(5):1071-1076. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  35. Laier C, Wegmann E, Brand M. Personality and cognition in gamers: Avoidance expectancies mediate the relationship between maladaptive personality traits and symptoms of Internet-gaming disorder. Front. Psychiatry 2018 Jul 10;9:304. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  36. Flay B, Petraitis J. A new theory of health behavior with implications for preventive interventions. Advances in medical sociology 1994;4:19-44.
  37. Grigsby TJ, Forster M, Unger JB, Sussman S. Predictors of alcohol-related negative consequences in adolescents: A systematic review of the literature and implications for future research. Journal of Adolescence 2016 Apr;48:18-35. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  38. Chun J. Determinants of tobacco use among Korean female adolescents: longitudinal test of the theory of triadic influence. Children and Youth Services Review 2015 Mar;50:83-87. [CrossRef]
  39. Klein VM. Cultural, social and intrapersonal factors associated with clusters of co-occurring health-related behaviours among adolescents. The European Journal of Public Health 2014;25(1):31-37. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  40. Hine DW, McKenzie-Richer A, Lewko J, Tilleczek K, Perreault L. A comparison of the mediational properties of four adolescent smoking expectancy measures. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors 2002;16(3):187-195. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  41. Ouellette JA, Gerrard M, Gibbons FX, Reis-Bergan M. Parents, peers, and prototypes: antecedents of adolescent alcohol expectancies, alcohol consumption, and alcohol-related life problems in rural youth. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors 1999;13(3):183-197. [CrossRef]
  42. National Bureau of Statistics of China. China Population Statistics Year Book of Year 2018. Beijing: National Bureau of Statistics of China; 2019.
  43. Petry NM, O'Brien CP. Internet gaming disorder and the DSM-5. Addiction 2013 Jul;108(7):1186-1187. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  44. Ko C, Yen J, Chen S, Wang P, Chen C, Yen C. Evaluation of the diagnostic criteria of Internet gaming disorder in the DSM-5 among young adults in Taiwan. J Psychiatr Res 2014 Jun;53:103-110. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  45. Sigerson L, Li AY, Cheung MW, Luk JW, Cheng C. Psychometric properties of the Chinese Internet Gaming Disorder Scale. Addict Behav 2017 Nov;74:20-26 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  46. Patton JH, Stanford MS, Barratt ES. Factor structure of the Barratt impulsiveness scale. J Clin Psychol 1995 Nov;51(6):768-774. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  47. Yan W. Psychometric properties of the Chinese version of the BIS-11 in three adolescent samples. Chinese Journal of Clinical Psychology 2016;24(5):869-873.
  48. Hu F, Chen G, Cai T. Preliminary Study on Self-Control Scale on Chinese Middle School Students. China Journal of Health Psychology 2012;20(8):1183-1184.
  49. Moss S, Prosser H, Costello H, Simpson N, Patel P, Rowe S, et al. Reliability and validity of the PAS-ADD Checklist for detecting psychiatric disorders in adults with intellectual disability. J Intellect Disabil Res 1998 Apr;42 ( Pt 2)(2):173-183. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  50. Hair JF. Multivariate Data Analysis. Hoboken, NJ: Prentice Hall; 2006.
  51. Yang X, Jiang X, Mo PK, Cai Y, Ma L, Lau JT. Prevalence and Interpersonal Correlates of Internet Gaming Disorders among Chinese Adolescents. Int J Environ Res Public Health 2020 Jan 16;17(2):579 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  52. Wu AMS, Chen JH, Tong K, Yu S, Lau JTF. Prevalence and associated factors of Internet gaming disorder among community dwelling adults in Macao, China. J Behav Addict 2018 Mar 01;7(1):62-69 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  53. Lemmens JS, Valkenburg PM, Peter J. The effects of pathological gaming on aggressive behavior. J Youth Adolesc 2011 Jan 13;40(1):38-47 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  54. Anderson CA, Sakamoto A, Gentile DA, Ihori N, Shibuya A, Yukawa S, et al. Longitudinal effects of violent video games on aggression in Japan and the United States. Pediatrics 2008 Nov 31;122(5):e1067-e1072. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  55. Király O, Griffiths MD, Urbán R, Farkas J, Kökönyei G, Elekes Z, et al. Problematic internet use and problematic online gaming are not the same: findings from a large nationally representative adolescent sample. Cyberpsychol Behav Soc Netw 2014 Dec;17(12):749-754 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  56. Wartberg L, Kriston L, Kammerl R. Associations of Social Support, Friends Only Known Through the Internet, and Health-Related Quality of Life with Internet Gaming Disorder in Adolescence. Cyberpsychol Behav Soc Netw 2017 Jul;20(7):436-441. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  57. Dong G, Zheng H, Liu X, Wang Y, Du X, Potenza MN. Gender-related differences in cue-elicited cravings in Internet gaming disorder: The effects of deprivation. J Behav Addict 2018 Dec 01;7(4):953-964 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  58. Ko C, Yen JY, Chen CC, Chen SH, Yen CF. Gender differences and related factors affecting online gaming addiction among Taiwanese adolescents. J Nerv Ment Dis 2005 Apr;193(4):273-277. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  59. Lopez-Fernandez O, Williams AJ, Griffiths MD, Kuss DJ. Female gaming, gaming addiction, and the role of women within gaming culture: a narrative literature review. Front Psychiatry 2019 Jul 10;10:454 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  60. Lemmens JS, Valkenburg PM, Gentile DA. The Internet Gaming Disorder Scale. Psychol Assess 2015 Jun;27(2):567-582. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  61. Rehbein F, Baier D. Family-, Media-, and School-Related Risk Factors of Video Game Addiction. Journal of Media Psychology 2013 Jan 01;25(3):118-128. [CrossRef]
  62. Przybylski AK, Weinstein N. Investigating the Motivational and Psychosocial Dynamics of Dysregulated Gaming: Evidence From a Preregistered Cohort Study. Clinical Psychological Science 2019 Aug 22;7(6):1257-1265. [CrossRef]
  63. Wang C, Chan CLW, Mak K, Ho S, Wong PWC, Ho RTH. Prevalence and correlates of video and internet gaming addiction among Hong Kong adolescents: a pilot study. ScientificWorldJournal 2014;2014:874648-874649 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  64. Rehbein F, Kliem S, Baier D, Mößle T, Petry NM. Prevalence of Internet gaming disorder in German adolescents: diagnostic contribution of the nine DSM-5 criteria in a state-wide representative sample. Addiction 2015 May 10;110(5):842-851. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  65. Kardefelt-Winther D. The moderating role of psychosocial well-being on the relationship between escapism and excessive online gaming. Computers in Human Behavior 2014 Sep;38:68-74. [CrossRef]
  66. Schneider LA, King DL, Delfabbro PH. Maladaptive coping styles in adolescents with Internet gaming disorder symptoms. Int J Ment Health Addiction 2017 Apr 20;16(4):905-916. [CrossRef]
  67. Schmitt ZL, Livingston MG. Video game addiction and college performance among males: results from a 1 year longitudinal study. Cyberpsychol Behav Soc Netw 2015 Jan;18(1):25-29. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  68. Ajzen I. The theory of planned behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 1991 Dec;50(2):179-211. [CrossRef]
  69. King DL, Herd MCE, Delfabbro PH. Tolerance in Internet gaming disorder: a need for increasing gaming time or something else? J Behav Addict 2017 Dec 01;6(4):525-533 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  70. Forman EM, Hoffman KL, McGrath KB, Herbert JD, Brandsma LL, Lowe MR. A comparison of acceptance- and control-based strategies for coping with food cravings: an analog study. Behav Res Ther 2007 Oct;45(10):2372-2386. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  71. Shiffman S. Coping with temptations to smoke. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 1984 Apr;52(2):261-267. [CrossRef]
  72. Gullo MJ, Dawe S. Impulsivity and adolescent substance use: rashly dismissed as "all-bad"? Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2008 Oct;32(8):1507-1518. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  73. Gullo M, Dawe S, Kambouropoulos N, Staiger PK, Jackson CJ. Alcohol expectancies and drinking refusal self-efficacy mediate the association of impulsivity with alcohol misuse. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 2010 Aug;34(8):1386-1399. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  74. Gullo MJ, Loxton NJ, Dawe S. Impulsivity: four ways five factors are not basic to addiction. Addict Behav 2014 Nov;39(11):1547-1556. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  75. Steinberg L, Chein JM. Multiple accounts of adolescent impulsivity. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2015 Jul 21;112(29):8807-8808 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  76. Steinberg L. A social neuroscience perspective on adolescent risk-taking. Dev Rev 2008 Mar;28(1):78-106 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  77. Litt MD, Kadden RM. Willpower versus "skillpower": Examining how self-efficacy works in treatment for marijuana dependence. Psychol Addict Behav 2015 Sep;29(3):532-540 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  78. Keller L, Bieleke M, Gollwitzer P. Mindset theory of action phases and if-then planning. In: Social Psychology in Action: Evidence-based Interventions from Theory to Practice. Cham: Springer; 2019:23-37.
  79. Prestwich A, Conner M, Lawton RJ. Implementation intentions: can they be used to prevent and treat addiction? In: Handbook of Implicit Cognition and Addiction. CA: Sage Publications; 2006:455-469.
  80. Zhou N, Fang X. Beyond peer contagion: Unique and interactive effects of multiple peer influences on Internet addiction among Chinese adolescents. Computers in Human Behavior 2015 Sep;50:231-238. [CrossRef]
  81. Torres-Rodríguez A, Griffiths MD, Carbonell X. The treatment of Internet gaming disorder: a brief overview of the PIPATIC program. Int J Ment Health Addict 2018 Nov 13;16(4):1000-1015 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  82. Apisitwasana N, Perngparn U, Cottler L. PRBM 2018 Apr;Volume 11:103-115. [CrossRef]
  83. Georgie J, Sean H, Deborah M, Matthew H, Rona C. Peer-led interventions to prevent tobacco, alcohol and/or drug use among young people aged 11-21 years: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Addiction 2016 Mar 09;111(3):391-407 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  84. Jo YS, Bhang SY, Choi JS, Lee HK, Lee SY, Kweon Y. Clinical characteristics of diagnosis for Internet Gaming Disorder: comparison of DSM-5 IGD and ICD-11 GD diagnosis. J Clin Med 2019 Jun 28;8(7):945 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]


CFI: comparative fit index
C-RIGCS: The Chinese version of the Revised internet Gaming Cognition Scale
DSM-5: Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, fifth edition
ICD-11: International Classification of Disease, eleventh revision
IGD: internet gaming disorder
MCIG: maladaptive cognitions related to internet gaming
ORc: crude odds ratio
RMSEA: root mean square error of approximation
SCT: social cognitive theory
SEM: structural equation modeling
TLI: Tucker-Lewis index
WHO: World Health Organization


Edited by R Kukafka; submitted 28.12.20; peer-reviewed by Y Miao, W Zhang; comments to author 26.03.21; revised version received 05.08.21; accepted 22.09.21; published 27.10.21

Copyright

©Yanqiu Yu, Phoenix Kit-Han Mo, Jianxin Zhang, Jibin Li, Joseph Tak-Fai Lau. Originally published in the Journal of Medical Internet Research (https://www.jmir.org), 27.10.2021.

This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work, first published in the Journal of Medical Internet Research, is properly cited. The complete bibliographic information, a link to the original publication on https://www.jmir.org/, as well as this copyright and license information must be included.