Published on in Vol 16, No 3 (2014): March

Economic Evaluation of a Web-Based Tailored Lifestyle Intervention for Adults: Findings Regarding Cost-Effectiveness and Cost-Utility From a Randomized Controlled Trial

Economic Evaluation of a Web-Based Tailored Lifestyle Intervention for Adults: Findings Regarding Cost-Effectiveness and Cost-Utility From a Randomized Controlled Trial

Economic Evaluation of a Web-Based Tailored Lifestyle Intervention for Adults: Findings Regarding Cost-Effectiveness and Cost-Utility From a Randomized Controlled Trial

Original Paper

1CAPHRI School for Public Health and Primary Care, Department of Health Promotion, Maastricht University, Maastricht, Netherlands

2Amsterdam School of Communication Research/ASCoR, Department of Communication Science, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, Netherlands

3Nutrition and Toxicology Research Institute Maastricht (NUTRIM), Department of Health Promotion, Maastricht University, Maastricht, Netherlands

4CAPHRI School for Public Health and Primary Care, Department of Health Services Research, Maastricht University, Maastricht, Netherlands

Corresponding Author:

Daniela N Schulz, MSc

CAPHRI School for Public Health and Primary Care

Department of Health Promotion

Maastricht University

P. Debyeplein 1

Maastricht, 6229HA

Netherlands

Phone: 31 433882832

Fax:31 433671032

Email: dn.schulz@maastrichtuniversity.nl


Background: Different studies have reported the effectiveness of Web-based computer-tailored lifestyle interventions, but economic evaluations of these interventions are scarce.

Objective: The objective was to assess the cost-effectiveness and cost-utility of a sequential and a simultaneous Web-based computer-tailored lifestyle intervention for adults compared to a control group.

Methods: The economic evaluation, conducted from a societal perspective, was part of a 2-year randomized controlled trial including 3 study groups. All groups received personalized health risk appraisals based on the guidelines for physical activity, fruit intake, vegetable intake, alcohol consumption, and smoking. Additionally, respondents in the sequential condition received personal advice about one lifestyle behavior in the first year and a second behavior in the second year; respondents in the simultaneous condition received personal advice about all unhealthy behaviors in both years. During a period of 24 months, health care use, medication use, absenteeism from work, and quality of life (EQ-5D-3L) were assessed every 3 months using Web-based questionnaires. Demographics were assessed at baseline, and lifestyle behaviors were assessed at both baseline and after 24 months. Cost-effectiveness and cost-utility analyses were performed based on the outcome measures lifestyle factor (the number of guidelines respondents adhered to) and quality of life, respectively. We accounted for uncertainty by using bootstrapping techniques and sensitivity analyses.

Results: A total of 1733 respondents were included in the analyses. From a willingness to pay of €4594 per additional guideline met, the sequential intervention (n=552) was likely to be the most cost-effective, whereas from a willingness to pay of €10,850, the simultaneous intervention (n=517) was likely to be most cost-effective. The control condition (n=664) appeared to be preferred with regard to quality of life.

Conclusions: Both the sequential and the simultaneous lifestyle interventions were likely to be cost-effective when it concerned the lifestyle factor, whereas the control condition was when it concerned quality of life. However, there is no accepted cutoff point for the willingness to pay per gain in lifestyle behaviors, making it impossible to draw firm conclusions. Further economic evaluations of lifestyle interventions are needed.

Trial Registration: Dutch Trial Register NTR2168; http://www.trialregister.nl/trialreg/admin/rctview.asp?TC=2168 (Archived by WebCite at http://www.webcitation.org/6MbUqttYB).

J Med Internet Res 2014;16(3):e91

doi:10.2196/jmir.3159

Keywords



Noncommunicable chronic diseases are associated with various modifiable health risk behaviors, such as physical inactivity, bad nutrition, excessive drinking, and smoking [1]. An unhealthy lifestyle and the consequences involved are related to a reduced quality of life [2] as well as substantial health care costs [3,4]. Stimulating a healthy lifestyle is important to improve health and prevent illness and to also reduce health care costs, especially with current budget cuts in the Netherlands and other countries [5,6].

Computer tailoring can be used successfully as an intervention to promote behaviors associated with a healthy lifestyle [7]. When applying computer tailoring, personalized feedback is generated by a computer program based on an individual assessment [8]. Earlier studies have demonstrated that tailored information is perceived as more relevant than nontailored information [9]. Moreover, computer-tailored interventions have proven to be effective in stimulating a healthier lifestyle, such as smoking cessation [10], preventing smoking relapse [11], encouraging healthy nutrition [12], lowering alcohol intake [13], and increasing physical activity [14]. Previous research has also indicated that changing multiple lifestyle-related behaviors is likely to be more effective than changing only a single behavior [15]. A recent study has shown that tailored interventions that aim to reduce multiple health risk behaviors are not only successful in reducing unhealthy behaviors but also in simultaneously enhancing the overall well-being of the individual [16]. The delivery of computer-tailored interventions targeting multiple health risk behaviors through the Internet has various benefits: These programs can be applied in privacy and at a time and place the respondent finds convenient, many people can be reached at relatively low intervention cost because more than 90% of the Dutch population has Internet access nowadays [17], and because the system is computerized it can be easily combined with and/or integrated in other programs or interventions.

Some economic evaluations of Web-based and/or computer-tailored programs have been conducted to date (eg, [18-23]). In general, these studies have given a first indication that these interventions—most were single behavior change interventions—can indeed be cost-effective. To our knowledge, however, no economic evaluation of a Web-based computer-tailored intervention targeting multiple health risk behaviors has been conducted so far.

Web-based computer-tailored lifestyle interventions are an interesting and promising option to make the health care system more sustainable because of their proven clinical effectiveness and their potential cost-effectiveness due to relatively low intervention costs and wide reach. Thus, information regarding the cost-effectiveness of Web-based computer-tailored intervention programs is crucial for health care decision makers and the government in making evidence-based decisions regarding large-scale implementation of such programs [24]. The aim of the present study, therefore, is to assess from a societal perspective the cost-effectiveness and cost-utility of 2 different versions (sequential and simultaneous) of a Web-based computer-tailored lifestyle intervention for adults compared to a control group that received only a minimal intervention.


Study Design and Participants

The economic evaluation was embedded in a 2-year single-blind randomized controlled trial including 3 study groups. The study was approved by the Medical Ethics Committee of Maastricht University and the University Hospital Maastricht (MEC 09-3-016/NL27235.068.09) and registered by the Dutch Trial Register (NTR2168).

In October 2009, the Dutch Regional Health Authorities of North-Brabant and Zeeland conducted the quadrennial Adult Health Monitor 2009 among inhabitants of these 2 provinces. This questionnaire could be completed on paper or online via the Internet. Respondents who completed the online version of the questionnaire were invited to take part in the present study. The Adult Health Monitor was interconnected with and integrated into our Web-based lifestyle intervention. The study website was also open to the general public; therefore, it was also possible to register for participation in the trial directly on the study website without having completed the Adult Health Monitor.

The inclusion period for this study was from November 2009 up to and including July 2010. The following inclusion criteria were used: aged between 18 and 65 years, having a computer with Internet access and basic Internet literacy, and having a valid email address. Participants were randomized into 1 of 2 experimental groups (sequential condition or simultaneous condition) or into the control condition, with an equal probability of being assigned to any of the 3 groups. Randomization took place at the individual level by means of a computer software randomization system. Figure 1 shows the Consolidated Standards of Reporting Trials (CONSORT) flow diagram.

Figure 1. Flow diagram of the economic study.
View this figure

Intervention

The intervention was a Web-based computer-tailored multisession program targeting adults. The primary aim of this lifestyle intervention was to motivate participants to be sufficiently physically active, to eat enough fruit and vegetables, to drink less alcohol, and to quit smoking. The intervention was based on the I-Change model, an integration of social cognitive models [25,26] and previously developed programs that have proven to be effective in increasing the level of physical activity [27], promoting the intake of fruit and vegetables [28], reducing the consumption of alcohol [29], and smoking cessation [30]. The respondents received feedback texts on their computer screens, which were aimed to motivate them to adopt the recommended health behaviors. All respondents received a health risk appraisal (HRA) indicating whether they adhered to the following public health guidelines: being moderately physically active for 30 minutes at least 5 days a week [31], eating 200 grams of vegetables per day [32], eating 2 pieces of fruit per day [32], not drinking more than 1 (for women) or 2 (for men) glasses of alcohol a day [32], and not smoking [33]. For all health risk behaviors, they received a traffic light image indicating whether they met (green), almost met (orange), or failed to meet (red) the guideline.

The experimental groups subsequently received personalized advice provided in 4 steps based on questions about different psychosocial determinants of the I-Change model [25,26]: (1) attitude, (2) social influence, (3) preparatory planning, and (4) self-efficacy and coping planning. At the end of every step, personal advice was given to participants. At baseline, respondents in the sequential condition could select a module concerning one of the lifestyle behaviors for which they did not meet the public health guidelines (ie, received a red or orange traffic light in their HRA); on completing this module, they received personalized feedback regarding this particular behavior. After 12 months, a second assessment took place and respondents had the opportunity to choose a second module and to receive feedback on a second lifestyle behavior for which they did not meet the public health guidelines. At baseline and after 12 months, respondents in the simultaneous condition received feedback on all the behaviors they did not meet the public health guideline for at one time. In both conditions, an overview of all received pieces of advice was available (via a link which was also sent by email) for the respondent at the end of the sessions. The control group received the HRA at baseline and after 24 months, but did not receive any additional personal advice. Figure 2 presents the design of the study, including all parts of the intervention. A detailed description of the study protocol has been published elsewhere [34].

Figure 2. Design of the study. HRA: health risk appraisal.
View this figure

Identification, Measurement, and Valuation of Costs and Effects

Overview

The economic evaluation was conducted from a societal perspective; therefore, all relevant costs (ie, intervention costs, health care costs, and respondent costs) and effects, such as quality-adjusted life years (QALYs) and lifestyle factor score (LFS), were taken into account [35].

Costs

Intervention costs consisted of hosting costs for the website, including costs for technical assistance and required updates. Costs for the development of the intervention program and research-specific costs were excluded because these are once-only costs that are not necessary again when implementing the program. The intervention costs were the same for all study groups because all groups received tailored advice that was integrated in the study website.

Health care costs included use of medication, medical consultations, inpatient and outpatient specialist care, hospital admissions, and other care (eg, professional home care). Health care costs were assessed using a 3-month retrospective questionnaire consisting of multiple choice and open-ended questions. This online questionnaire was taken quarterly during the 24 months. The updated Dutch Manual for Cost Analysis in Health Care was used to valuate costs [36]. If cost prices were not available, other sources were used. For instance, the website of the Healthcare Insurance Board [37] was used to calculate medication costs. The costs of medications were calculated based on the dose described by the respondent. Hence, costs per tablet, gram, or milliliter were used to calculate total medication costs for each respondent. Costs for health care services that could not be found in the Dutch Manual for Cost Analysis in Health Care were looked up on the Internet (eg, via the websites of health care services). If possible, 3 costs for each health care service were looked up to ultimately calculate a mean cost price for this service. Cost price details can be found in Multimedia Appendix 1.

Productivity costs included costs because of sickness absenteeism from work. They were calculated by the human capital method using mean costs for the Dutch population corrected for gender and age [36].

Respondent costs (also known as patient and family costs) included the time respondents spent on the website for participation and costs for traveling to health care services. For the time spent on the website, we used the mean time that was necessary to complete the program within the 3 study groups. The time lost due to participation in the Adult Health Monitor was also taken into account, but we only added the time people needed to fill out the parts of the Adult Health Monitor regarding the 5 lifestyle behaviors. We made this decision for 2 reasons: Firstly, it was not necessary to combine the interventions and the entire Monitor of the Regional Health Authorities; secondly, respondents who participated in the Adult Health Monitor skipped these parts on our website, whereas people who did not take part in the Adult Health Monitor completed these questions on our website (ie, in the end, all respondents completed the same number of questions). Ultimately, we used an average time of 70 minutes for the sequential condition, 100 minutes for the simultaneous condition, and 20 minutes for the control condition. To determine the cost of time spent on the website, we valued the time by applying the labor time using the mean costs of the Dutch population corrected for gender and age [36]. Costs for traveling to health care services were also valued in monetary terms. These costs were assessed based on average travel distances to health care services in the Netherlands [38] and the mean costs per kilometer [36].

Effects

For the cost-effectiveness analysis, the primary outcome measure was the total LFS. The following questionnaires were used to assess the 5 lifestyle behaviors: the Short Questionnaire to Assess Health-enhancing physical activity (SQUASH) [39,40], a 4-item Food Frequency Questionnaire (FFQ) assessing weekly fruit and fruit juice intake [39], a 4-item FFQ assessing the weekly consumption of boiled or baked vegetables as well as salads or raw vegetables [39], the 5-item Dutch Quantity-Frequency-Variability (QFV) questionnaire to assess alcohol intake [39,41], and questions asking participants if they smoked, what they smoked (cigarettes, cigars, or pipe tobacco), and how much they smoked per day (cigarettes) or per week (cigars or pipe tobacco) [39]. Based on the guidelines for physical activity, fruit intake, vegetable intake, alcohol consumption, and smoking, we calculated this LFS by summing all healthy behaviors (ie, complying with the guideline in question)—a similar method (Prudence score) was applied by Parekh et al [42]—at baseline and after 24 months; thus, the value of the LFS could range from 0 (adhering to no guidelines) to 5 (adhering to all guidelines). Moreover, a lifestyle factor change index (LFCI) was calculated by subtracting the LFS at baseline from the LFS at 24 months [43]. The value for this index could range from -5 to +5 on a continuous 10-point scale; positive scores indicated an increase, whereas negative scores indicated a decrease in the number of healthy behaviors.

For the cost-utility analysis, the primary outcome measure was utilities based on a health-related quality of life instrument. The Euro-Qol EQ-5D-3L questionnaire [44] was used to assess health-related quality of life and was completed by respondents every 3 months. The EQ-5D-3L consists of the following 5 health dimensions: mobility, self-care, daily activity, pain/discomfort, and anxiety/depression. On a 3-point Likert scale, respondents had to indicate their own state of health (no complaints=1; some complaints=2; many complaints=3). A utility score was calculated for each measurement point using the Dutch tariff [45]. This score could range from -0.33 (death) to 1 (perfect health). This utility score, in turn, was used to calculate the QALYs gained or lost during the 2-year study period by making use of the area under the curve method. The area under the curve stands for the duration of the health state (x-axis, 24 months) multiplied by the quality weight for the health state (y-axis, utility score).

Indexing and Discounting

The price year of this study was 2013. All cost prices were indexed to the year 2013 by using the consumer price indexes of 105.38 for the year 2009, 106.72 for the year 2010, 109.22 for the year 2011, 111.90 for the year 2012, and 115.00 for the year 2013 [46]. Because of the long-term follow-up of 2 years, costs made in the second year were discounted by 4%, and effects regarding the LFS assessed after 24 months and effects in QALYs measured in the second year of the study were discounted by 1.5% [36,47].

Statistical Analyses

Respondents were included in the analyses when their LFS at baseline was available and when the economic evaluation measurement was completed at least at baseline.

To examine whether randomization was successful and whether the 3 groups were comparable in terms of demographics, baseline LFS, quality of life, and health care costs over the previous 3 months, analyses of variance (ANOVA) were used for continuous variables and chi-square tests for dichotomous or categorical variables. To investigate whether selective dropout had occurred, logistic regression analyses were used to compare (1) those who took part in the intervention but did not complete the questionnaires needed for the economic evaluation with those who did complete these questionnaires and were included in this study, and (2) of those included in this study, those who filled out at least 1 follow-up questionnaire regarding the economic evaluation with those who did not fill out any of the follow-up questionnaires.

Intention-to-treat analyses were performed. Mean imputation was used to fill in missing values regarding medication use, health care services, absenteeism from work, EQ-5D-3L items, and lifestyle items. When applying mean imputation, the mean of the previous and next value for the same variable was calculated. If mean imputation was impossible because of missing values on multiple measurement points, the last observation carried forward (LOCF) method was used to fill in missing values [48]. The next observation carried backward method was used when the value was not available on the baseline questionnaire. Unrealistic/impossible values (eg, more than 90 days absent from work in a period of 90 days) were recoded as the highest possible value. In case of unclear answers to the open-ended question regarding medication use (eg, private, too much, I do not know anymore), mean prices of the study group for this question were imputed.

To compare the 3 study groups regarding their biennial costs (ie, health care costs and respondent costs over a period of 2 years), nonparametric bootstrapping (5000 times) with 95% confidence intervals in percentiles was used. ANOVAs were performed to compare the groups regarding the LFS assessed after 24 months and the QALYs measured over the study period of 2 years.

Incremental costs (in Euros) and effects were calculated for all 3 study groups as well as a net monetary benefit by valuing the effectiveness and utility outcomes in monetary values using a threshold for society’s willingness to pay (WTP) per gain in the LFS (ie, per additional guideline met) and per QALY gained [49]. The probability of the highest net monetary benefit was presented from a WTP of €0 to €80,000 [50]. Additionally, we explicitly reported the probabilities when using a WTP of €18,000 because this is an accepted Dutch cutoff point per QALY gained as a result of preventive interventions [50].

Uncertainty Analyses

For the cost-effectiveness and cost-utility analyses, bootstrapping resampling techniques (with 1000 times replacement) were carried out to deal with uncertainty around the estimates of cost-effectiveness and cost-utility. The results were presented in cost-effectiveness and cost-utility acceptability curves. Seven different sensitivity analyses were performed to deal with the uncertainty of parameter estimates from the primary analysis: (1) we executed the analyses from a health care perspective by excluding the productivity costs and the respondent costs (these might be reflected in participants’ reported quality of life anyway) [36]; (2) we excluded costs due to absenteeism from work because these costs differed significantly between the 3 study groups before the intervention; (3) we excluded respondents with less than 4 follow-up measurement points because of the large number of missing values (>50%); (4) we used a LFS change index as outcome variable to correct for the LFS before the intervention (cost-effectiveness analysis) and corrected the QALY for baseline utility (cost-utility analysis) [51]; (5) we excluded respondents with the highest costs based on the 95th percentile; (6) we did not discount costs and effect outcomes [36,47]; and (7) we discounted both the costs and effect outcomes by 4.0% instead of discounting only costs by 4.0% and effects by 1.5% [47].

Bootstrap analyses were done using Microsoft Office Excel 2010; all other analyses were done using SPSS version 20.0 (IBM Corp, Armonk, NY, USA).


Sample Characteristics

A total of 1733 respondents were included in the analyses. Table 1 presents the baseline characteristics of the 3 study groups. One baseline difference was found between the groups: the sequential condition had significantly higher costs because of absenteeism from work compared to the control group.

Table 1. Comparability of the 3 study groups regarding demographics, health status, lifestyle behavior, and health care and travel costs over the past 3 months before baseline (N=1733)
VariableSequential (n=552)Simultaneous (n=517)Control (n=664)F (df)χ2 (df)P
Age, mean (SD)47.31 (11.62)48.15 (11.96)48.88 (12.19)2.61 (2, 1730)
.07
Gender, n (%)






Men279 (50.5)264 (51.1)366 (55.1)



Women273 (49.5)253 (48.9)298 (44.9)
3.1 (2).21
Educational level, n (%)






Low53 (9.9)64 (12.9)90 (13.7)



Medium274 (51.3)222 (44.7)319 (48.7)



High207 (38.8)211 (42.5)246 (37.6)
7.9 (4).10
Income per month (€),an (%)






< 1,750123 (22.9)123 (24.7)161 (24.5)



1750 - 3050265 (49.3)249 (50.0)341 (52.0)



> 3050150 (27.9)126 (25.3)154 (23.5)
3.2 (4).52
Employment situation, n (%)






Job (paid)389 (72.2)350 (70.3)443 (67.4)



No job150 (27.8)148 (29.7)214 (32.6)
3.2 (2).20
Marital status, n (%)






Relationship423 (78.9)402 (80.2)507 (77.4)



Single113 (21.1)99 (19.8)148 (22.6)
1.4 (2).50
Persons in household, mean (SD)2.83 (1.65)2.69 (1.24)2.71 (1.37)1.54 (2, 1695)
.21
Native country, n (%)






The Netherlands511 (95.0)480 (96.0)627 (95.3)



Other27 (5.0)20 (4.0)31 (4.7)
0.6 (2).73
BMI, mean (SD)25.70 (4.18)25.38 (3.91)25.80 (5.71)1.56 (2, 1714)
.21
Psychological distress, mean (SD)44.46 (6.40)45.00 (5.84)44.74 (6.10)1.00 (2, 1687)
.37
Diseases,bn (%)






Diabetes25 (4.7)20 (4.0)23 (3.5)
1.0 (2).61

Brain hemorrhage, TIA2 (0.4)4 (0.8)7 (1.1)
1.9 (2).38

Heart attack8 (1.5)5 (1.0)5 (0.8)
1.5 (2).48

Other serious heart disease4 (0.7)12 (2.4)15 (2.3)
5.2 (2).07

Cancer12 (2.2)7 (1.4)11 (1.7)
1.1 (2).58

High blood pressure67 (12.5)67 (13.4)112 (17.2)
6.0 (2).05

Asthma, COPD36 (6.7)34 (6.8)43 (6.6)
0.01 (2).99
Lifestyle factor, mean (SD)3.26 (1.04)3.32 (1.07)3.27 (1.06)0.52 (2, 1730)
.59
Euroqol/utility (EQ-5D-3L), mean (SD)0.89 (0.17)0.89 (0.15)0.90 (0.15)0.40 (2, 1617)
.67
Health care costs (€),cmean (SD)






Medication use44.62 (256.77)39.14 (313.81)31.68 (204.78)0.39 (2, 1725)
.68

Health care service119.12 (401.90)114.42 (201.50)99.16 (184.52)0.88 (2, 1729)
.42

Admissions32.95 (231.26)31.98 (222.70)62.85 (422.57)1.88 (2, 1728)
.15

Other care34.04 (220.28)21.28 (157.88)40.41 (265.29)1.08 (2, 1728)
.34
Absenteeism from work676.68 (3135.31)405.10 (2076.48)328.19 (1413.07)3.63 (2, 1674)
.03
Travel costs (€), mean (SD)3.20 (10.42)3.46 (6.41)2.97 (5.63)0.54 (2, 1579)
.59

aRespondents who did not want to report their income (n=257) were classified in the category €1751-€3050.

bTIA: transient ischemic attack; COPD: chronic obstructive pulmonary disease.

cCosts for prior 3 months.

Dropout Analyses

Results from the logistic regression analysis showed that respondents who were excluded from the analyses of the present study (ie, those who took part in the intervention, but did not complete the questionnaires needed for the economic evaluation, n=3322) were significantly younger (beta=.03; P<.001), had a lower LFS (beta=–.12; P<.001), reported less diseases such as brain hemorrhage and transient ischemic attack (TIA) (beta=1.14; P=.04), and more of them did not have a paid job (beta=.40; P<.001) than respondents included in our analyses.

Each of the 8 retrospective questionnaires for the previous 3 months required for the economic evaluation was completed by at least 36.47% (632/1733) and not more than 57.76% (1001/1733) of the respondents, whereas 506 of 1733 respondents (29.20%) did not complete any of these questionnaires. The latter were younger (beta=.02; P=.002) and reported an unhealthier lifestyle (beta=–.12; P=.02) than those who completed at least 1 questionnaire.

After imputing missing values, total cost data was available for 1662 of 1733 respondents (95.90%), and effect data was available for all 1733 respondents (100%) for the LFS and for 1690 of 1733 respondents (97.52%) for QALYs.

Costs and Effects

Table 2 shows that the simultaneous condition reported statistically significantly higher costs for health care services during the 2-year period than the control condition did. However, no differences were found regarding the total biennial health care costs. The travel costs were also statistically significantly higher for the simultaneous condition than the control condition. Because the simultaneous condition was the most time-intensive condition, followed by the sequential condition and then the control condition, total respondent costs were also higher among the simultaneous condition compared with the sequential and control conditions, and higher among the sequential condition compared to the control condition. Table 3 demonstrates that there were no differences found between the 3 study groups regarding effects on the LFS or on QALYs. Detailed information regarding the lifestyle scores at the different time points and effects of the intervention on the 5 behaviors are presented elsewhere [52].

Table 2. Mean biennial costs per participant per study group based on 5000 bootstrap replications.
Cost typeCosts per study group (€), mean (SD)95% CI

SequentialSimultaneousControlSequential-SimultaneousControl-SimultaneousSequential-Control
Intervention costs






Fixed hosting costs0.540.540.54


Health care costs






Medication use (n=1733)193 (57)200 (74)162 (31)-208.73, 166.37-222.27, 91.00-81.95, 175.03

Health care service (n=1733)719 (124)861 (73)641 (42)-385.33, 169.11-386.86, -56.07-131.59, 368.62

Admissions (n=1732)396 (89)303 (78)461 (97)-141.58, 325.48-87.86, 401.07-327.51, 200.20

Other care (n=1732)222 (67)178 (40)247 (67)-97.69, 204.61-76.51, 230.00-211.27, 161.91

Total health care costs (n=1714)1521 (195)1534 (162)1502 (151)-508.11, 504.92-461.19, 399.62-453.30, 493.35
Productivity costs






Absenteeism (n=1714)3489 (652)3563 (666)2600 (538)-1889.89, 1751.48-2642.42, 673.86-724.22, 2586.62
Respondent costs






Travel costs (n=1678)25 (3)30 (3)24 (2)-13.26, 3.42-12.55, -0.41-5.40, 9.10

Fixed time costs (n=1733)41 (0)59 (1)12 (0)-18.65, -16.27-47.71, -45.7128.53, 29.95

Total respondent costs (n=1678)64 (3)87 (3)35 (2)-30.66, -13.28-57.81, -45.3922.25, 37.19
Table 3. Mean biennial effect on the lifestyle factor score (LFS) and quality-adjusted life year (QALY) score per participant per study group.
EffectMean (SD) per study groupF (df)P

SequentialSimultaneousControl

LFSa (n=1733)3.37 (1.08)3.42 (1.06)3.34 (1.05)0.92 (2, 1730).40
QALY (EQ-5D-3L) (n=1690)1.80 (0.30)1.78 (0.31)1.82 (0.27)1.79 (2, 1687).17

aThis factor could range from 0 (adherence to all guidelines) to 5 (adherence to no guideline at all), plus discounting effect.

Cost-Effectiveness Analyses

Table 4 shows that the costs in the control condition were less than the costs in the sequential and the simultaneous conditions, but that the effects in the 2 experimental conditions were higher. From a WTP of €4594 or more, the sequential condition appeared more likely to be cost-effective than the control condition. From a WTP of €10,850 or more, the simultaneous condition seemed more likely to be cost-effective than the control condition. When comparing the cost-effectiveness of the 2 experimental conditions, the simultaneous condition seemed more likely to be cost-effective than the sequential condition from a WTP of €17,106.

The probability that the sequential condition was cost-effective at a WTP of €0 per gain in lifestyle score was 42%. For the simultaneous condition, the probability was 10% and for the control condition, the probability was 48% (see Table 5 and Figure 3). With a WTP of €18,000 per gain in lifestyle score, the simultaneous intervention would probably (ie, 45%) be the most cost-effective, followed by the sequential intervention (ie, 39%). The 2 sensitivity analyses performed with different discounts confirmed this finding. Results based on the other sensitivity analyses were slightly different. The sensitivity analysis using a LFCI as outcome variable (ie, correcting for the baseline lifestyle score) showed that the sequential condition might be most likely to be most cost-effective independent of the WTP. According to the sensitivity analysis performed from a health care perspective, the sensitivity analysis from which costs because of work absenteeism were excluded and the sensitivity analysis from which respondents with extremely high costs were excluded, both experimental conditions were found to be more likely to be cost-effective than the control condition regardless of the WTP. Based on the sensitivity analysis with inclusion of respondents who filled out the follow-up questionnaires at least 4 times (50%), the sequential condition was shown to be less likely to be cost-effective than the simultaneous and control condition for a WTP up to €9000.

Table 4. Incremental costs and effects per gain in lifestyle factor score (LFS) and per quality-adjusted life year (QALY) gained for the study groups with a willingness-to-pay threshold of €18,000.
InterventionIncremental costs (€)aIncremental effectbIncremental costs per LFS/QALY (€)
Lifestyle factor



Sequential vs control183.76.044594.00

Simultaneous vs control868.00.0810,850.00

Sequential vs simultaneous–684.24–.0417,106.00
QALY (EQ-5D-3L)



Sequential vs control183.76–.01Dominatedc

Simultaneous vs control868.00–.03Dominatedc

Sequential vs simultaneous–684.24.02Dominatedc

aCosts per participant (€): sequential=4324.76; simultaneous=5009.00; control=4141.00.

bLifestyle factor effects per participant: sequential=3.38, simultaneous=3.42, control=3.34; QALYs effects per participant: sequential=1.80, simultaneous=1.78, control=1.81.

cIn one group, the costs are higher and the effects are lower compared to the other group.

Table 5. Results from cost-effectiveness and cost-utility analyses based on 1000 bootstrap replications for the sequential (seq), simultaneous (sim), and control (con) conditions.
Type of analysisStudy group (n)Probability of highest net monetary benefit (%)

SeqSimConWTP=€0WTP=€18,000WTP=€80,000




SeqSimConSeqSimConSeqSimCon
Primary analysis












LFS55251766442104839451626686

QALY (EQ-5D-3L)535501654391249214767192
Sensitivity analyses LFS












Only health care costs included5525176643827352470624716

Productivity costs Excluded5525176645920212271720737

≥ 50% of follow-up questionnaires completed100104195329681483220800

LFCI to correct for baseline scores552517664451145651520632710

Respondents with outliers on total costs excludeda525488637811182569622726

Without discounting costs and effects55251766441114837451826668

Discounting costs and effects 2nd year with 4.0%55251766443114639451627667
Sensitivity analyses QALY (EQ-5D-3L)












Only health care costs included535501654313238103877192

Productivity costs excluded535501654561925142847193

≥ 50% of follow-up questionnaires completed100104195625705217472173

QALY corrected for baseline as outcome variable51348262138184336135129863

Respondents with outliers on total costs excludeda509475627809112537313285

Without discounting costs and effects535501654381250224758191

Discounting costs and effects 2nd year with 4.0%535501654401248224757192

aBased on the 95th percentile: €18,567.64.

Figure 3. Cost-effectiveness acceptability curve for the study groups based on primary and sensitivity analyses.
View this figure

Cost-Utility Analyses

With regard to cost-utility, Table 4 shows that both the sequential and the simultaneous condition were dominated by the control condition. This means that the costs of the control condition were lower and the effects with regard to QALYs gained were higher in this group. When comparing the sequential condition with the simultaneous condition, costs were lower and effects on QALYs were larger in the sequential condition. Thus, the simultaneous condition was dominated by the sequential condition.

The probability that the sequential condition was most efficient at a WTP of €0 per QALY gained was 39%. For the simultaneous condition, the probability was 12% and for the control condition, the probability was 49% (see Table 5 and Figure 4). With a WTP of €18,000 per QALY gained, the cost-utility analysis showed that the control condition would probably (ie, 76%) be the most efficient condition. Indeed, Figure 4 shows that the control condition was most likely to be efficient, irrespective of the WTP. All but 3 sensitivity analyses confirmed these results. The sensitivity analysis in which costs due to work absenteeism were excluded and the sensitivity analysis in which respondents with extremely high costs were excluded showed that with a WTP up to €2980 and €7900, respectively, the sequential condition would probably be the most efficient (ie, >45% and >48% chance, respectively). The sensitivity analysis that included only those respondents who filled out the follow-up questionnaires at least 4 times (50% of all questionnaires) showed that the simultaneous condition was likely to be more efficient than the sequential condition, independent of the WTP. In all other analyses, this was the other way around.

Figure 4. Cost-utility acceptability curves for the study groups based on primary and sensitivity analyses.
View this figure

Principal Findings

An economic evaluation of 2 different versions of a Web-based computer-tailored multiple lifestyle intervention was performed. Despite some variations in the different sensitivity analyses outcomes, the results of this study give an indication that the 2 tailored intervention programs are likely to be more cost-effective when looking at lifestyles as a primary outcome than that of a control group, in which respondents received a short tailored overview. In general, the simultaneous intervention was likely to be most cost-effective, followed by the sequential intervention. However, the results were sensitive to baseline scores. When correcting for lifestyle behavior at baseline, the sequential intervention was probably most cost-effective. Regarding cost-utilities, the intervention received by the control group might be most preferable when compared to both lifestyle interventions (sequential and simultaneous).

With incremental costs of €4594 per gain in lifestyle score by meeting additional public health guidelines, the sequential condition is most likely to be cost-effective. With incremental costs of €17,106 or higher, the simultaneous version of the Web-based intervention is even more likely to be cost-effective than the sequential version. The incremental costs of our intervention seemed to be less favorable than the costs of €160 per guideline met in the study by Van Keulen et al [22]. However, the studies are hard to compare because the intervention tested by Van Keulen and colleagues [22] only considered 3 lifestyle behaviors (ie, physical activity, fruit intake, and vegetable intake) and tailored print communication was used instead of Web-based communication. Their study population consisted of older adults (aged 45-70 years). Most importantly, their control group received no intervention at all, whereas in our study the control group received a minimal intervention. Information regarding one’s lifestyle behavior may be sufficient to facilitate change and improve health risk behaviors [42]. Although the effects might be modest, in the present study the difference in effects between the control group who received the minimal intervention and the experimental conditions who received the sequential and simultaneous versions of the Web-based interventions might have become smaller. In a single behavior change intervention aimed at smoking cessation, Smit et al [21] reported incremental costs of €5100 per abstinent participant. This amount is comparable to our findings regarding gains in lifestyle behaviors, including smoking cessation. However, both studies [21,22] used shorter study periods (1.5 years and 1 year, respectively). This means that the cost-effectiveness regarding outcomes measured at the last follow-up in this study, which was after 2 years, cannot truly be compared with the cost-effectiveness results found in those studies.

Our finding that the control group might be most preferable regarding cost-utilities is comparable to the results reported by Smit et al [21] who found that the usual care their control group received was probably the most preferable intervention when compared with a Web-based computer-tailoring intervention and a combination of the Web-based intervention and face-to-face counseling by a practice nurse. A likely explanation for this finding may be that the follow-up period of 2 years was too short to find effects on quality of life. Although the intervention leads to lifestyle improvements that may prevent or postpone the incidence of a variety of lifestyle-related diseases, health gains from prevention programs often only become noticeable many years after the costs are made [53]. Moreover, baseline scores on the EQ-5D-3L were already high at the beginning of the study (mean 0.90, SD 0.15), which might be related to a restriction of this measurement tool (ie, the ceiling effect: the tool does not differentiate between high scores of the healthy utility range [54]). Consequently, the finding that our study population consisted of people reporting high scores on the EQ-5D-3L may be an explanation for not finding any statistically significant differences in QALYs gained between the groups. For public health interventions, it might be better to use other outcomes related to quality of life (eg, nonhealth outcomes, such as empowerment or satisfaction), which are more sensitive to changes in the short term because these quality of life measures tend to underestimate the relative benefits of this kind of intervention [55]. However, 2 cost-effectiveness acceptability curves (sensitivity analyses in which productivity costs were excluded and in which respondents with extremely high total costs were excluded) showed that the sequential condition might be preferable up to a WTP of €2700 and €8600, respectively, which is opposite to the findings by Van Keulen et al [22], whose control group was probably the most cost-effective intervention for ratios lower than €2851 per QALY gained.

In the literature, a WTP of €18,000 is the accepted cutoff point per QALY gained [50]. However, there is no such cutoff point regarding lifestyle changes as used in our study. This makes the interpretation of the results regarding cost-effectiveness complicated. Lifestyle interventions usually aim at preventing different kinds of diseases with different burdens. This makes it difficult to determine such a cutoff point, something that has also been pointed out for increases in smoking abstinence rates [21] or for each kilogram of body weight lost [56], for example. As suggested by Tate et al [24], for future research it would be good to transform the unit changes of different outcome measures into metrics that can be compared across different kinds of interventions (regardless of the target behavior). Thus, future research should aim to define a WTP cutoff point for different lifestyle behaviors or metrics that can be used for different lifestyle behaviors.

Furthermore, there has been discussion about the rates of discounting effects (eg, [36,57,58]), especially in the field of prevention. Effect outcomes should be discounted because the value of QALYs or health increases with time [59,60] and this value change is not taken into account in economic evaluations [61]. It has been argued that the same value of discounting should be used for costs and effects to be consistent [62], whereas it has been recommended that effects should be discounted at lower rates to correct for the increasing monetary value of health over time [36,47,53]. In our study, we reported the results without discounting, with 1.5% discounting, and with 4.0% discounting based on the guidelines for pharmacoeconomic research [47]. The similarity of the findings provides evidence for the robustness of our results.

The results revealed that respondents in the simultaneous condition reported higher costs because of health care service use during the 2-year study period than did respondents in the control condition. The travel costs in this group were also higher. These costs might be related to the number of visits to caregivers. The advice may have served as a kind of prompt among the respondents to ask caregivers for help in improving their lifestyle (eg, for smoking cessation guidance) [21]. However, it remains unclear why these costs were higher among respondents in the simultaneous intervention. It is conceivable that the marginal differences between the groups occurred because of measurement errors.

Strengths and Limitations

To our knowledge, this was the first study assessing cost-effectiveness and cost-utility of 2 versions of a Web-based tailored lifestyle intervention aiming at multiple behavior change. Two outcome measures (ie, a LFS and QALYs) were used to evaluate costs and intervention effects over a relatively long period of 2 years. Seven different sensitivity analyses based on different views of economic evaluations found in the literature [36,51,57,62] were performed to test the robustness of the results. This is a further strong point of this study, although some analyses showed slightly different results.

There are some limitations that should be kept in mind when interpreting the results. First, we compared our intervention groups to a control group that also received a small amount of tailored information (ie, a personalized HRA). Because this HRA was integrated in the study website, the intervention costs were the same for all study groups. This strategy may have inflated the results and it may be that the cost-effectiveness would have been better if a different control group—who received either general information or no information at all—was used. Second, although we used a large sample (N=1733), the study suffered from high dropout rates, a common phenomenon in Web-based intervention studies (eg, [63-65]); therefore, many missing values for the follow-up assessments had to be imputed. Consequently, the imputation procedure may have distorted the reliability of the findings to some extent. That is, when using the conservative LOCF method to fill in the missing data for a large part of the sample, finding any intervention effects becomes more unlikely. The high dropout rates might have been caused by the need to assess health care costs and quality of life on a relatively large number of occasions. Although it might be good to measure health care use, medication use, absenteeism from work, and quality of life every 3 months to counteract recall bias [66], this may also be too time-consuming for some participants. This may have resulted in most of the respondents not completing all questionnaires and others dropping out of the intervention. Thus, future studies should aim to prevent loss to follow-up by sending tailored reminder emails [67], for example.

As presented in the dropout analysis, selective dropout occurred. Sensitivity analyses were performed to provide a more complete picture of the results among this selective group. Despite randomization of respondents to 1 of 3 study groups, statistically significant differences were found with regard to productivity costs, and some differences almost reached statistical significance (ie, age, high blood pressure, and serious heart diseases). These differences may have influenced the results. Also, we assessed absenteeism from work, but not “presenteeism” [68]. Finally, we used self-reported questionnaires that are subject to bias. Additional objective measures, such as medication registration at pharmacies and data from insurance companies, and cost diaries [69] could be included in future studies.

Conclusions

Computer-tailored advice on lifestyle behaviors was likely to be cost-effective after 24 months when looking at lifestyles as a primary outcome. The Web-based tailored lifestyle intervention using a simultaneous approach is promising as the most cost-effective intervention in improving someone’s lifestyle, followed by the intervention using a sequential approach. However, with regard to improving quality of life, the control condition seemed to be preferable. Future studies should aim to compare different computer-tailoring conditions to a control group that does not receive any personalized advice, and to identify a cutoff point for the WTP for lifestyle changes.

Acknowledgments

This study was funded by ZonMw, the Netherlands Organization for Health Research and Development (grant number: 120610012). The authors wish to thank the project partners at the Regional Health Authorities of the Dutch provinces of North-Brabant (GGD Brabant-Zuidoost; GGD Hart voor Brabant; and GGD West-Brabant) and Zeeland (GGD Zeeland) for their assistance in implementing the intervention and recruiting the participants. The authors wish to thank Mathieu van Adrichem, Joris Peters, and Anika Thielmann for data cleaning/data entry assistance.

Conflicts of Interest

Hein de Vries is scientific director of Vision2Health, a company that licenses evidence-based innovative computer-tailored health communication tools. No other authors reported any conflicts of interest.

Multimedia Appendix 1

Cost prices for the different types of health care costs and absenteeism from work.

PDF File (Adobe PDF File), 8KB

Multimedia Appendix 2

CONSORT-EHEALTH checklist [70].

PDF File (Adobe PDF File), 996KB

  1. Ezzati M, Hoorn SV, Rodgers A, Lopez AD, Mathers CD, Murray CJ, Comparative Risk Assessment Collaborating Group. Estimates of global and regional potential health gains from reducing multiple major risk factors. Lancet 2003 Jul 26;362(9380):271-280. [Medline]
  2. Davies CA, Vandelanotte C, Duncan MJ, van Uffelen JG. Associations of physical activity and screen-time on health related quality of life in adults. Prev Med 2012 Jul;55(1):46-49. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  3. von Lengerke T, Krauth C. Economic costs of adult obesity: a review of recent European studies with a focus on subgroup-specific costs. Maturitas 2011 Jul;69(3):220-229. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  4. Rappange DR, Brouwer WB, Hoogenveen RT, Van Baal PH. Healthcare costs and obesity prevention: drug costs and other sector-specific consequences. Pharmacoeconomics 2009;27(12):1031-1044. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  5. Landelijke nota gezondheidsbeleid National Government, 2011. Gezondheid dichtbij Health nearby   URL: http:/​/www.​rijksoverheid.nl/​bestanden/​documenten-en-publicaties/​notas/​2011/​05/​25/​landelijke-nota-gezondheidsbeleid/​landelijke-nota-gezondheidsbeleid.​pdf [accessed 2013-12-01] [WebCite Cache]
  6. Karanikolos M, Mladovsky P, Cylus J, Thomson S, Basu S, Stuckler D, et al. Financial crisis, austerity, and health in Europe. Lancet 2013 Apr 13;381(9874):1323-1331. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  7. Krebs P, Prochaska JO, Rossi JS. A meta-analysis of computer-tailored interventions for health behavior change. Prev Med 2010;51(3-4):214-221 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  8. de Vries H, Brug J. Computer-tailored interventions motivating people to adopt health promoting behaviours: introduction to a new approach. Patient Educ Couns 1999 Feb;36(2):99-105. [Medline]
  9. Kreuter MW, Wray RJ. Tailored and targeted health communication: strategies for enhancing information relevance. Am J Health Behav 2003 Dec;27 Suppl 3:S227-S232. [Medline]
  10. Te Poel F, Bolman C, Reubsaet A, de Vries H. Efficacy of a single computer-tailored e-mail for smoking cessation: results after 6 months. Health Educ Res 2009 Dec;24(6):930-940 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  11. Elfeddali I, Bolman C, Candel MJ, Wiers RW, de Vries H. Preventing smoking relapse via Web-based computer-tailored feedback: a randomized controlled trial. J Med Internet Res 2012;14(4):e109 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  12. Oenema A, Tan F, Brug J. Short-term efficacy of a web-based computer-tailored nutrition intervention: main effects and mediators. Ann Behav Med 2005 Feb;29(1):54-63. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  13. Riper H, Kramer J, Smit F, Conijn B, Schippers G, Cuijpers P. Web-based self-help for problem drinkers: a pragmatic randomized trial. Addiction 2008 Feb;103(2):218-227. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  14. Spittaels H, De Bourdeaudhuij I, Vandelanotte C. Evaluation of a website-delivered computer-tailored intervention for increasing physical activity in the general population. Prev Med 2007 Mar;44(3):209-217. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  15. Prochaska JO. Multiple Health Behavior Research represents the future of preventive medicine. Prev Med 2008 Mar;46(3):281-285. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  16. Prochaska JO, Evers KE, Castle PH, Johnson JL, Prochaska JM, Rula EY, et al. Enhancing multiple domains of well-being by decreasing multiple health risk behaviors: a randomized clinical trial. Popul Health Manag 2012 Oct;15(5):276-286. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  17. Internet World Stats. 2012. Internet usage in Europe: Internet user statistics and population for 53 European countries and regions   URL: http://www.internetworldstats.com/stats4.htm [accessed 2013-12-01] [WebCite Cache]
  18. Cobiac LJ, Vos T, Barendregt JJ. Cost-effectiveness of interventions to promote physical activity: a modelling study. PLoS Med 2009 Jul 14;6(7):e1000110 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  19. Gerhards SA, de Graaf LE, Jacobs LE, Severens JL, Huibers MJ, Arntz A, et al. Economic evaluation of online computerised cognitive-behavioural therapy without support for depression in primary care: randomised trial. Br J Psychiatry 2010 Apr;196(4):310-318 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  20. Smit F, Lokkerbol J, Riper H, Majo MC, Boon B, Blankers M. Modeling the cost-effectiveness of health care systems for alcohol use disorders: how implementation of eHealth interventions improves cost-effectiveness. J Med Internet Res 2011;13(3):e56 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  21. Smit ES, Evers SM, de Vries H, Hoving C. Cost-effectiveness and cost-utility of Internet-based computer tailoring for smoking cessation. J Med Internet Res 2013;15(3):e57 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  22. van Keulen HM, Bosmans JE, van Tulder MW, Severens JL, de Vries H, Brug J, et al. Cost-effectiveness of tailored print communication, telephone motivational interviewing, and a combination of the two: results of an economic evaluation alongside the Vitalum randomized controlled trial. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 2010;7:64 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  23. Warmerdam L, Smit F, van Straten A, Riper H, Cuijpers P. Cost-utility and cost-effectiveness of internet-based treatment for adults with depressive symptoms: randomized trial. J Med Internet Res 2010;12(5):e53 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  24. Tate DF, Finkelstein EA, Khavjou O, Gustafson A. Cost effectiveness of internet interventions: review and recommendations. Ann Behav Med 2009 Aug;38(1):40-45 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  25. de Vries H, Mudde A, Leijs I, Charlton A, Vartiainen E, Buijs G, et al. The European Smoking Prevention Framework Approach (EFSA): an example of integral prevention. Health Educ Res 2003 Oct;18(5):611-626 [FREE Full text] [Medline]
  26. de Vries H, Eggers SM, Bolman C. The role of action planning and plan enactment for smoking cessation. BMC Public Health 2013;13:393 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  27. Smeets T, Brug J, de Vries H. Effects of tailoring health messages on physical activity. Health Educ Res 2008 Jun;23(3):402-413 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  28. Oenema A, Tan F, Brug J. Short-term efficacy of a web-based computer-tailored nutrition intervention: main effects and mediators. Ann Behav Med 2005 Feb;29(1):54-63. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  29. Riper H, Kramer J, Smit F, Conijn B, Schippers G, Cuijpers P. Web-based self-help for problem drinkers: a pragmatic randomized trial. Addiction 2008 Feb;103(2):218-227. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  30. Dijkstra A, De Vries H, Roijackers J, van Breukelen G. Tailored interventions to communicate stage-matched information to smokers in different motivational stages. J Consult Clin Psychol 1998 Jun;66(3):549-557. [Medline]
  31. Hildebrandt VH, Chorus AMJ, Stubbe JH. Trendrapport Bewegen en Gezondheid 2008-2009. Leiden: TNO Kwaliteit van Leven; 2010.
  32. Gezondheidsraad [The Health Council of the Netherlands]. Richtlijnen goede voeding 2006–achtergronddocument [Guidelines for good nutrition]. Den Haag: Gezondheidsraad; 2006.
  33. Kwaliteitsinstituut voor de Gezondheidszorg CBO [The Dutch Institute for Healthcare Improvement]. Richtlijn behandeling van tabaksverslaving [Guidelines for treating tobacco addiction]. Utrecht/Den Haag: BVO/Partnership Stop met Roken; 2004.
  34. Schulz DN, Kremers SP, van Osch LA, Schneider F, van Adrichem MJ, de Vries H. Testing a Dutch web-based tailored lifestyle programme among adults: a study protocol. BMC Public Health 2011;11:108 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  35. Drummond MF. Methods for the Economic Evaluation of Health Care Programmes. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 1997.
  36. Hakkaart-van Roijen L, Tan SS, Bouwmans C. Handleiding voor kostenonderzoek: Methoden en standaard kostprijzen voor economische evaluaties in de gezondheidszorg kostenonderzoek. 2010.   URL: http:/​/www.​cvz.nl/​binaries/​content/​documents/​zinl-www/​documenten/​publicaties/​overige-publicaties/​1007-handleiding-voor-kostenonderzoek/​Handleiding+voor+kostenonderzoek.​pdf [accessed 2013-07-30] [WebCite Cache]
  37. College van zorgverzekeringen [Healthcare Insurance Board]. Medicijnkosten. 2013.   URL: http://www.medicijnkosten.nl/ [accessed 2013-12-01] [WebCite Cache]
  38. Westert GP, Van den Berg MJ, Koolman X, Verkleij H. Zorgbalans 2008. De prestatie van de Nederlandse gezondheidszorg. RIVM: Bilthoven; 2008.
  39. National Institute for Public Health and the Environment (RIVM). Indicatoren voor de Monitor Volksgezondheid Local and national health monitoring. Lokale en nationale Monitor gezondheid   URL: https://www.monitorgezondheid.nl/volksindicatoren.aspx [accessed 2014-03-12] [WebCite Cache]
  40. Wendel-Vos GC, Schuit AJ, Saris WH, Kromhout D. Reproducibility and relative validity of the short questionnaire to assess health-enhancing physical activity. J Clin Epidemiol 2003 Dec;56(12):1163-1169. [Medline]
  41. Lemmens P, Tan ES, Knibbe RA. Measuring quantity and frequency of drinking in a general population survey: a comparison of five indices. J Stud Alcohol 1992 Sep;53(5):476-486. [Medline]
  42. Parekh S, King D, Owen N, Jamrozik K. Spousal concordance and reliability of the 'Prudence Score' as a summary of diet and lifestyle. Aust N Z J Public Health 2009 Aug;33(4):320-324. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  43. Carlson JA, Sallis JF, Ramirez ER, Patrick K, Norman GJ. Physical activity and dietary behavior change in Internet-based weight loss interventions: comparing two multiple-behavior change indices. Prev Med 2012 Jan;54(1):50-54 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  44. EuroQol Group. EuroQol--a new facility for the measurement of health-related quality of life. Health Policy 1990 Dec;16(3):199-208. [Medline]
  45. Lamers LM, Stalmeier PF, McDonnell J, Krabbe PF, van Busschbach JJ. [Measuring the quality of life in economic evaluations: the Dutch EQ-5D tariff]. Ned Tijdschr Geneeskd 2005 Jul 9;149(28):1574-1578. [Medline]
  46. Centraal Bureau voor de Statistiek (CBS). Consumentenprijzen. Prijsindex 2006. 2013.   URL: http:/​/statline.​cbs.nl/​StatWeb/​publication/​?VW=T&DM=SLNL&PA=71311NED&D1=0,2,4,6&D2=0-1,61,70,87,108,137,145,172,176,221-222,230,255,l&D3=(l-34)-l&HD=081020-1310&HDR=T&STB=G1,G2 [WebCite Cache]
  47. College voor zorgverzekeringen. Richtlijnen voor farmaco-economisch onderzoek; geactualiseerde versie Guidelines for pharmacoeconomic research. 2006.   URL: http:/​/www.​cvz.nl/​binaries/​content/​documents/​zinl-www/​documenten/​rubrieken/​pakket/​pakketbeheer/​0604-richtlijnen-voor-farmaco-economisch-onderzoek/​Richtlijnen+voor+farmaco-economisch+onderzoek.​pdf [accessed 2013-12-01] [WebCite Cache]
  48. Hendriks MR, Al MJ, Bleijlevens MH, van Haastregt JC, Crebolder HF, van Eijk JT, et al. Continuous versus intermittent data collection of health care utilization. Med Decis Making 2013 Nov;33(8):998-1008. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  49. Stinnett AA, Mullahy J. Net health benefits: a new framework for the analysis of uncertainty in cost-effectiveness analysis. Med Decis Making 1998;18(2 Suppl):S68-S80. [Medline]
  50. RVZ. 2006. Zinnige en duurzame zorg   URL: http://www.rvz.net/uploads/docs/Advies_-_Zinnige_en_duurzame_zorg.pdf [accessed 2013-12-01] [WebCite Cache]
  51. Manca A, Hawkins N, Sculpher MJ. Estimating mean QALYs in trial-based cost-effectiveness analysis: the importance of controlling for baseline utility. Health Econ 2005 May;14(5):487-496. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  52. Schulz DN, Kremers SP, Vandelanotte C, van Adrichem MJ, Schneider F, Candel MJ, et al. Effects of a web-based tailored multiple-lifestyle intervention for adults: a two-year randomized controlled trial comparing sequential and simultaneous delivery modes. J Med Internet Res 2014;16(1):e26 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  53. Brouwer WBF, Rutten FFH. De hoogte van de disconteervoeten in economische evaluaties: Is de tijd rijp voor een nieuwe richtlijn? Institute for Medical Technology Assessment. 2005.   URL: http://www.bmg.eur.nl/fileadmin/ASSETS/bmg/english/iMTA/Publications/Reports/200573.pdf [accessed 2013-12-01] [WebCite Cache]
  54. Kopec JA, Willison KD. A comparative review of four preference-weighted measures of health-related quality of life. J Clin Epidemiol 2003 Apr;56(4):317-325. [Medline]
  55. Lorgelly PK, Lawson KD, Fenwick EA, Briggs AH. Outcome measurement in economic evaluations of public health interventions: a role for the capability approach? Int J Environ Res Public Health 2010 May;7(5):2274-2289 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  56. van Wier MF, Dekkers JC, Bosmans JE, Heymans MW, Hendriksen IJ, Pronk NP, et al. Economic evaluation of a weight control program with e-mail and telephone counseling among overweight employees: a randomized controlled trial. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 2012;9:112 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  57. Bonneux L, Birnie E. The discount rate in the economic evaluation of prevention: a thought experiment. J Epidemiol Community Health 2001 Feb;55(2):123-125 [FREE Full text] [Medline]
  58. Claxton K, Paulden M, Gravelle H, Brouwer W, Culyer AJ. Discounting and decision making in the economic evaluation of health-care technologies. Health Econ 2011 Jan;20(1):2-15. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  59. Gravelle H, Smith D. Discounting for health effects in cost-benefit and cost-effectiveness analysis. Health Econ 2001 Oct;10(7):587-599. [Medline]
  60. Gravelle H, Brouwer W, Niessen L, Postma M, Rutten F. Discounting in economic evaluations: stepping forward towards optimal decision rules. Health Econ 2007 Mar;16(3):307-317. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  61. Brouwer WB, Niessen LW, Postma MJ, Rutten FF. Need for differential discounting of costs and health effects in cost effectiveness analyses. BMJ 2005 Aug 20;331(7514):446-448 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  62. Weinstein MC, Stason WB. Foundations of cost-effectiveness analysis for health and medical practices. N Engl J Med 1977 Mar 31;296(13):716-721. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  63. Crutzen R. Hard to Get, Hard to Keep: Dissemination of and Exposure to Internet-Delivered Health Behavior Change Interventions Aimed at Adolescents [PhD dissertation]. Maastricht: Maastricht University; 2009.
  64. Eysenbach G. The law of attrition. J Med Internet Res 2005;7(1):e11 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  65. Glasgow RE, Nelson CC, Kearney KA, Reid R, Ritzwoller DP, Strecher VJ, et al. Reach, engagement, and retention in an Internet-based weight loss program in a multi-site randomized controlled trial. J Med Internet Res 2007;9(2):e11 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  66. Davis JC, Bryan S, Marra CA, Sharma D, Chan A, Beattie BL, et al. An economic evaluation of resistance training and aerobic training versus balance and toning exercises in older adults with mild cognitive impairment. PLoS One 2013;8(5):e63031 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  67. Schneider F, van Osch L, Schulz DN, Kremers SP, de Vries H. The influence of user characteristics and a periodic email prompt on exposure to an internet-delivered computer-tailored lifestyle program. J Med Internet Res 2012;14(2):e40 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  68. Hemp P. Presenteeism: at work--but out of it. Harv Bus Rev 2004 Oct;82(10):49-58, 155. [Medline]
  69. Goossens ME, Rutten-van Mölken MP, Vlaeyen JW, van der Linden SM. The cost diary: a method to measure direct and indirect costs in cost-effectiveness research. J Clin Epidemiol 2000 Jul;53(7):688-695. [Medline]
  70. Eysenbach G, CONSORT-EHEALTH Group. CONSORT-EHEALTH: improving and standardizing evaluation reports of Web-based and mobile health interventions. J Med Internet Res 2011;13(4):e126 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]


CONSORT: Consolidated Standards of Reporting Trials
FFQ: Food Frequency Questionnaire
HRA: health risk appraisal
LFCI: lifestyle factor change index
LFS: lifestyle factor score
LOCF: last observation carried forward
QALY: quality-adjusted life year
QFV: Quantity-Frequency-Variability
TIA: transient ischemic attack
WTP: willingness to pay


Edited by G Eysenbach; submitted 04.12.13; peer-reviewed by M Blankers, L Warmerdam; comments to author 23.12.13; revised version received 02.02.14; accepted 27.02.14; published 20.03.14

Copyright

©Daniela N Schulz, Eline S Smit, Nicola E Stanczyk, Stef PJ Kremers, Hein de Vries, Silvia MAA Evers. Originally published in the Journal of Medical Internet Research (http://www.jmir.org), 20.03.2014.

This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work, first published in the Journal of Medical Internet Research, is properly cited. The complete bibliographic information, a link to the original publication on http://www.jmir.org/, as well as this copyright and license information must be included.